Grad Coach

Research Topics & Ideas: Neuroscience

50 Topic Ideas To Kickstart Your Research Project

Neuroscience research topics and ideas

If you’re just starting out exploring neuroscience-related topics for your dissertation, thesis or research project, you’ve come to the right place. In this post, we’ll help kickstart your research by providing a hearty list of neuroscience-related research ideas , including examples from recent studies.

PS – This is just the start…

We know it’s exciting to run through a list of research topics, but please keep in mind that this list is just a starting point . These topic ideas provided here are intentionally broad and generic , so keep in mind that you will need to develop them further. Nevertheless, they should inspire some ideas for your project.

To develop a suitable research topic, you’ll need to identify a clear and convincing research gap , and a viable plan to fill that gap. If this sounds foreign to you, check out our free research topic webinar that explores how to find and refine a high-quality research topic, from scratch. Alternatively, consider our 1-on-1 coaching service .

Research topic idea mega list

Neuroscience-Related Research Topics

  • Investigating the neural mechanisms underlying memory consolidation during sleep.
  • The role of neuroplasticity in recovery from traumatic brain injury.
  • Analyzing the impact of chronic stress on hippocampal function.
  • The neural correlates of anxiety disorders: A functional MRI study.
  • Investigating the effects of meditation on brain structure and function in mindfulness practitioners.
  • The role of the gut-brain axis in the development of neurodegenerative diseases.
  • Analyzing the neurobiological basis of addiction and its implications for treatment.
  • The impact of prenatal exposure to environmental toxins on neurodevelopment.
  • Investigating gender differences in brain aging and the risk of Alzheimer’s disease.
  • The neural mechanisms of pain perception and its modulation by psychological factors.
  • Analyzing the effects of bilingualism on cognitive flexibility and brain aging.
  • The role of the endocannabinoid system in regulating mood and emotional responses.
  • Investigating the neurobiological underpinnings of obsessive-compulsive disorder.
  • The impact of virtual reality technology on cognitive rehabilitation in stroke patients.
  • Analyzing the neural basis of social cognition deficits in autism spectrum disorders.
  • The role of neuroinflammation in the progression of multiple sclerosis.
  • Investigating the effects of dietary interventions on brain health and cognitive function.
  • The neural substrates of decision-making under risk and uncertainty.
  • Analyzing the impact of early life stress on brain development and mental health outcomes.
  • The role of dopamine in motivation and reward processing in the human brain.
  • Investigating neural circuitry changes in depression and response to antidepressants.
  • The impact of sleep deprivation on cognitive performance and neural function.
  • Analyzing the brain mechanisms involved in empathy and moral reasoning.
  • The role of the prefrontal cortex in executive function and impulse control.
  • Investigating the neurophysiological basis of schizophrenia.

Research topic evaluator

Neuroscience Research Ideas (Continued)

  • The impact of chronic pain on brain structure and connectivity.
  • Analyzing the effects of physical exercise on neurogenesis and cognitive aging.
  • The neural mechanisms underlying hallucinations in psychiatric and neurological disorders.
  • Investigating the impact of music therapy on brain recovery post-stroke.
  • The role of astrocytes in neural communication and brain homeostasis.
  • Analyzing the effect of hormone fluctuations on mood and cognition in women.
  • The impact of neurofeedback training on attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD).
  • Investigating the neural basis of resilience to stress and trauma.
  • The role of the cerebellum in non-motor cognitive and affective functions.
  • Analyzing the contribution of genetics to individual differences in brain structure and function.
  • The impact of air pollution on neurodevelopment and cognitive decline.
  • Investigating the neural mechanisms of visual perception and visual illusions.
  • The role of mirror neurons in empathy and social understanding.
  • Analyzing the neural correlates of language development and language disorders.
  • The impact of social isolation on neurocognitive health in the elderly.
  • Investigating the brain mechanisms involved in chronic fatigue syndrome.
  • The role of serotonin in mood regulation and its implications for antidepressant therapies.
  • Analyzing the neural basis of impulsivity and its relation to risky behaviors.
  • The impact of mobile technology usage on attention and brain function.
  • Investigating the neural substrates of fear and anxiety-related disorders.
  • The role of the olfactory system in memory and emotional processing.
  • Analyzing the impact of gut microbiome alterations on central nervous system diseases.
  • The neural mechanisms of placebo and nocebo effects.
  • Investigating cortical reorganization following limb amputation and phantom limb pain.
  • The role of epigenetics in neural development and neurodevelopmental disorders.

Recent Neuroscience Studies

While the ideas we’ve presented above are a decent starting point for finding a research topic, they are fairly generic and non-specific. So, it helps to look at actual studies in the neuroscience space to see how this all comes together in practice.

Below, we’ve included a selection of recent studies to help refine your thinking. These are actual studies,  so they can provide some useful insight as to what a research topic looks like in practice.

  • The Neurodata Without Borders ecosystem for neurophysiological data science (Rübel et al., 2022)
  • Genetic regulation of central synapse formation and organization in Drosophila melanogaster (Duhart & Mosca, 2022)
  • Embracing brain and behaviour: Designing programs of complementary neurophysiological and behavioural studies (Kirwan et al., 2022).
  • Neuroscience and Education (Georgieva, 2022)
  • Why Wait? Neuroscience Is for Everyone! (Myslinski, 2022)
  • Neuroscience Knowledge and Endorsement of Neuromyths among Educators: What Is the Scenario in Brazil? (Simoes et al., 2022)
  • Design of Clinical Trials and Ethical Concerns in Neurosciences (Mehanna, 2022) Methodological Approaches and Considerations for Generating Evidence that Informs the Science of Learning (Anderson, 2022)
  • Exploring the research on neuroscience as a basis to understand work-based outcomes and to formulate new insights into the effective management of human resources in the workplace: A review study (Menon & Bhagat, 2022)
  • Neuroimaging Applications for Diagnosis and Therapy of Pathologies in the Central and Peripheral Nervous System (Middei, 2022)
  • The Role of Human Communicative Competence in Post-Industrial Society (Ilishova et al., 2022)
  • Gold nanostructures: synthesis, properties, and neurological applications (Zare et al., 2022)
  • Interpretable Graph Neural Networks for Connectome-Based Brain Disorder Analysis (Cui et al., 2022)

As you can see, these research topics are a lot more focused than the generic topic ideas we presented earlier. So, for you to develop a high-quality research topic, you’ll need to get specific and laser-focused on a specific context with specific variables of interest.  In the video below, we explore some other important things you’ll need to consider when crafting your research topic.

Get 1-On-1 Help

If you’re still unsure about how to find a quality research topic, check out our Research Topic Kickstarter service, which is the perfect starting point for developing a unique, well-justified research topic.

Research Topic Kickstarter - Need Help Finding A Research Topic?

You Might Also Like:

Topic Kickstarter: Research topics in education

Submit a Comment Cancel reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

Save my name, email, and website in this browser for the next time I comment.

  • Print Friendly

Recent advances in neurotechnologies with broad potential for neuroscience research

Affiliations.

  • 1 Center for Bio-Integrated Electronics, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, USA.
  • 2 Department of Mechanical Engineering, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, USA.
  • 3 Department of Materials Science and Engineering, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, USA.
  • 4 Center for Bio-Integrated Electronics, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, USA. [email protected].
  • 5 Department of Mechanical Engineering, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, USA. [email protected].
  • 6 Department of Materials Science and Engineering, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, USA. [email protected].
  • 7 Department of Biomedical Engineering, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, USA. [email protected].
  • 8 Department of Electrical Engineering and Computer Science, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, USA. [email protected].
  • 9 Querrey Simpson Institute for Bioelectronics, Northwestern University, Chicago, IL, USA. [email protected].
  • 10 Department of Neurological Surgery, Feinberg School of Medicine, Northwestern University, Chicago, IL, USA. [email protected].
  • PMID: 33199897
  • DOI: 10.1038/s41593-020-00739-8

Interest in deciphering the fundamental mechanisms and processes of the human mind represents a central driving force in modern neuroscience research. Activities in support of this goal rely on advanced methodologies and engineering systems that are capable of interrogating and stimulating neural pathways, from single cells in small networks to interconnections that span the entire brain. Recent research establishes the foundations for a broad range of creative neurotechnologies that enable unique modes of operation in this context. This review focuses on those systems with proven utility in animal model studies and with levels of technical maturity that suggest a potential for broad deployment to the neuroscience community in the relatively near future. We include a brief summary of existing and emerging neuroscience techniques, as background for a primary focus on device technologies that address associated opportunities in electrical, optical and microfluidic neural interfaces, some with multimodal capabilities. Examples of the use of these technologies in recent neuroscience studies illustrate their practical value. The vibrancy of the engineering science associated with these platforms, the interdisciplinary nature of this field of research and its relevance to grand challenges in the treatment of neurological disorders motivate continued growth of this area of study.

Publication types

  • Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't
  • Brain / physiology
  • Electric Stimulation / instrumentation
  • Electric Stimulation / methods
  • Neurosciences / methods
  • Neurosciences / trends*
  • Optogenetics / trends
  • Pharmacology / methods
  • Technology / trends*
  • Reference Manager
  • Simple TEXT file

People also looked at

Review article, attention in psychology, neuroscience, and machine learning.

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

  • Gatsby Computational Neuroscience Unit, Sainsbury Wellcome Centre, University College London, London, United Kingdom

Attention is the important ability to flexibly control limited computational resources. It has been studied in conjunction with many other topics in neuroscience and psychology including awareness, vigilance, saliency, executive control, and learning. It has also recently been applied in several domains in machine learning. The relationship between the study of biological attention and its use as a tool to enhance artificial neural networks is not always clear. This review starts by providing an overview of how attention is conceptualized in the neuroscience and psychology literature. It then covers several use cases of attention in machine learning, indicating their biological counterparts where they exist. Finally, the ways in which artificial attention can be further inspired by biology for the production of complex and integrative systems is explored.

1. Introduction

Attention is a topic widely discussed publicly and widely studied scientifically. It has many definitions within and across multiple fields including psychology, neuroscience, and, most recently, machine learning ( Chun et al., 2011 ; Cho et al., 2015 ). As William James wrote at the dawn of experimental psychology, “Everyone knows what attention is. It is the taking possession by the mind, in clear, and vivid form, of one out of what seems several simultaneously possible objects or trains of thought.” Since James wrote this, many attempts have been made to more precisely define and quantify this process while also identifying the underlying mental and neural architectures that give rise to it. The glut of different experimental approaches and conceptualizations to study what is spoken of as a single concept, however, has led to something of a backlash amongst researchers. As was claimed in the title of a recent article arguing for a more evolution-informed approach to the concept, “No one knows what attention is” ( Hommel et al., 2019 ).

Attention is certainly far from a clear or unified concept. Yet despite its many, vague, and sometimes conflicting definitions, there is a core quality of attention that is demonstrably of high importance to information processing in the brain and, increasingly, artificial systems. Attention is the flexible control of limited computational resources. Why those resources are limited and how they can best be controlled will vary across use cases, but the ability to dynamically alter and route the flow of information has clear benefits for the adaptiveness of any system.

The realization that attention plays many roles in the brain makes its addition to artificial neural networks unsurprising. Artificial neural networks are parallel processing systems comprised of individual units designed to mimic the basic input-output function of neurons. These models are currently dominating the machine learning and artificial intelligence (AI) literature. Initially constructed without attention, various mechanisms for dynamically re-configuring the representations or structures of these networks have now been added.

The following section, section 2, will cover broadly the different uses of the word attention in neuroscience and psychology, along with its connection to other common neuroscientific topics. Throughout, the conceptualization of attention as a way to control limited resources will be highlighted. Behavioral studies will be used to demonstrate the abilities and limits of attention while neural mechanisms point to the physical means through which these behavioral effects are manifested. In section 3, the state of attention research in machine learning will be summarized and relationships between artificial and biological attention will be indicated where they exist. And in section 4 additional ways in which findings from biological attention can influence its artificial counterpart will be presented.

The primary aim of this review is to give researchers in the field of AI or machine learning an understanding of how attention is conceptualized and studied in neuroscience and psychology in order to facilitate further inspiration where fruitful. A secondary aim is to inform those who study biological attention how these processes are being operationalized in artificial systems as it may influence thinking about the functional implications of biological findings.

2. Attention in Neuroscience and Psychology

The scientific study of attention began in psychology, where careful behavioral experimentation can give rise to precise demonstrations of the tendencies and abilities of attention in different circumstances. Cognitive science and cognitive psychology aim to turn these observations into models of how mental processes could create such behavioral patterns. Many word models and computational models have been created that posit different underlying mechanisms ( Driver, 2001 ; Borji and Itti, 2012 ).

The influence of single-cell neurophysiology in non-human primates along with non-invasive means of monitoring human brain activity such as EEG, fMRI, and MEG have made direct observation of the underlying neural processes possible. From this, computational models of neural circuits have been built that can replicate certain features of the neural responses that relate to attention ( Shipp, 2004 ).

In the following sub-sections, the behavioral and neural findings of several different broad classes of attention will be discussed.

2.1. Attention as Arousal, Alertness, or Vigilance

In its most generic form, attention could be described as merely an overall level of alertness or ability to engage with surroundings. In this way it interacts with arousal and the sleep-wake spectrum. Vigilance in psychology refers to the ability to sustain attention and is therefore related as well. Note, while the use of these words clusters around the same meaning, they are sometimes used more specifically in different niche literature ( Oken et al., 2006 ).

Studying subjects in different phases of the sleep-wake cycle, under sleep deprivation, or while on sedatives offers a view of how this form of attention can vary and what the behavioral consequences are. By giving subjects repetitive tasks that require a level of sustained attention—such as keeping a ball within a certain region on a screen—researchers have observed extended periods of poor performance in drowsy patients that correlate with changes in EEG signals ( Makeig et al., 2000 ). Yet, there are ways in which tasks can be made more engaging that can lead to higher performance even in drowsy or sedated states. This includes increasing the promise of reward for performing the task, adding novelty or irregularity, or introducing stress ( Oken et al., 2006 ). Therefore, general attention appears to have limited reserves that won't be deployed in the case of a mundane or insufficiently rewarding task but can be called upon for more promising or interesting work.

Interestingly, more arousal is not always beneficial. The Yerkes-Dodson curve ( Figure 1B ) is an inverted-U that represents performance as a function of alertness on sufficiently challenging tasks: at low levels of alertness performance is poor, at medium levels it is good, and at high levels it becomes poor again. The original study used electric shocks in mice to vary the level of alertness, but the finding has been repeated with other measures ( Diamond, 2005 ). It may explain why psychostimulants such as Adderall or caffeine can work to increase focus in some people at some doses but become detrimental for others ( Wood et al., 2014 ).

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1 . General attention and alertness (A) Cells in the locus coeruleus release norepinephrine (also known as noradrenaline) onto many parts of the brain with different functions, including onto other neuromodulatory systems. This contributes to overall arousal ( Samuels and Szabadi, 2008 ). Colors here represent different divisions of the brain: forebrain (green), diencephalon (yellow), and brainstem (blue). (B) The Yerkes-Dodson curve describes the nonlinear relationship between arousal and performance on challenging tasks.

The neural circuits underlying the sleep-wake cycle are primarily in the brain stem ( Coenen, 1998 ). These circuits control the flow of information into the thalamus and then onto cortex. Additionally, neuromodulatory systems play a large role in the control of generalized attention. Norepinephrine, acetylcholine, and dopamine are believed to influence alertnesss, orienting to important information, and executive control of attention, respectively ( Posner, 2008 ). The anatomy of neuromodulators matches their function as well. Neurons that release norepinephrine, for example, have their cell bodies in the brain stem but project very broadly across the brain, allowing them to control information processing broadly ( Figure 1A ).

2.2. Sensory Attention

In addition to overall levels of arousal and alertness, attention can also be selectively deployed by an awake subject to specific sensory inputs. Studying attention within the context of a specific sensory system allows for tight control over both stimuli and the locus of attention. Generally, to look for this type of attention the task used needs to be quite challenging. For example, in a change detection task, the to-be-detected difference between two stimuli may be very slight. More generally, task difficulty can be achieved by presenting the stimulus for only a very short period of time or only very weakly.

A large portion of the study of attention in systems neuroscience and psychology centers on visual attention in particular ( Kanwisher and Wojciulik, 2000 ). This may reflect the general trend in these fields to emphasis the study of visual processing over other sensory systems ( Hutmacher, 2019 ), along with the dominant role vision plays in the primate brain. Furthermore, visual stimuli are frequently used in studies meant to address more general, cognitive aspects of attention as well.

Visual attention can be broken down broadly into spatial and feature-based attention.

2.2.1. Visual Spatial Attention

Saccades are small and rapid eye movements made several times each second. As the fovea offers the highest visual resolution on the retina, choosing where to place it is essentially a choice about where to deploy limited computational resources. In this way, eye movements indicate the locus of attention. As this shift of attention is outwardly visible it is known as overt visual attention.

By tracking eye movements as subjects are presented with different images, researchers have identified image patterns that automatically attract attention. Such patterns are defined by oriented edges, spatial frequency, color contrast, intensity, or motion ( Itti and Koch, 2001 ). Image regions that attract attention are considered “salient” and are computed in a “bottom-up” fashion. That is, they don't require conscious or effortful processing to identify and are likely the result of built-in feature detectors in the visual system. As such, saliency can be computed very quickly. Furthermore, different subjects tend to agree on which regions are salient, especially those identified in the first few saccades ( Tatler et al., 2005 ).

Salient regions can be studied in “free-viewing” situations, that is, when the subject is not given any specific instructions about how to view the image. When a particular task is assigned, the interplay between bottom-up and “top-down” attention becomes clear. For example, when instructed to saccade to a specific visual target out of an array, subjects may incorrectly saccade to a particularly salient distractor instead ( van Zoest and Donk, 2005 ). More generally, task instructions can have a significant effect on the pattern of saccades generated when subjects are viewing a complex natural image and given high-level tasks (e.g., asked to assess the age of a person or guess their socio-economic status). Furthermore, the natural pattern of eye movements when subjects perform real world tasks, like sandwich making, can provide insights to underlying cognitive processes ( Hayhoe and Ballard, 2005 ).

When subjects need to make multiple saccades in a row they tend not to return to locations they have recently attended and may be slow to respond if something relevant occurs there. This phenomenon is known as inhibition of return ( Itti and Koch, 2001 ). Such behavior pushes the visual system to not just exploit image regions originally deemed most salient but to explore other areas as well. It also means the saccade generating system needs to have a form of memory; this is believed to be implemented by short-term inhibition of the representation of recently-attended locations.

While eye movements are an effective means of controlling visual attention, they are not the only option. “Covert” spatial attention is a way of emphasizing processing of different spatial locations without an overt shift in fovea location. Generally, in the study of covert spatial attention, subjects must fixate on a central point throughout the task. They are cued to covertly attend to a location in their peripheral vision where stimuli relevant for their visual task will likely appear. For example, in an orientation discrimination task, after the spatial cue is provided an oriented grating will flash in the cued location and the subject will need to indicate its orientation. On invalidly-cued trials (when the stimulus appears in an uncued location), subjects perform worse than on validly-cued (or uncued) trials ( Anton-Erxleben and Carrasco, 2013 ). This indicates that covert spatial attention is a limited resource that can be flexibly deployed and aids in the processing of visual information.

Covert spatial attention is selective in the sense that certain regions are selected for further processing at the expense of others. This has been referred to as the “spotlight” of attention. Importantly, for covert—as opposed to overt—attention the input to the visual system can be identical while the processing of that input is flexibly selective.

Covert spatial attention can be impacted by bottom-up saliency as well. If an irrelevant but salient object is flashed at a location that then goes on to have a task relevant stimulus, the exogenous spatial attention drawn by the irrelevant stimulus can get applied to the task relevant stimulus, possibly providing a performance benefit. If it is flashed at an irrelevant location, however, it will not help, and can harm performance ( Berger et al., 2005 ). Bottom-up/exogenous attention has a quick time course, impacting covert attention for 80–130 ms after the distractor appears ( Anton-Erxleben and Carrasco, 2013 ).

In some theories of attention, covert spatial attention exists to help guide overt attention. Particularly, the pre-motor theory of attention posits that the same neural circuits plan saccades and control covert spatial attention ( Rizzolatti et al., 1987 ). The frontal eye field (FEF) is known to be involved in the control of eye movements. Stimulating the neurons in FEF at levels too low to evoke eye movements has been shown to create effects similar to covert attention ( Moore et al., 2003 ). In this way, covert attention may be a means of deciding where to overtly look. The ability to covertly attend may additionally be helpful in social species, as eye movements convey information about knowledge and intent that may best be kept secret ( Klein et al., 2009 ).

To study the neural correlates of covert spatial attention, researchers identify which aspects of neural activity differ based only on differences in the attentional cue (and not on differences in bottom-up features of the stimuli). On trials where attention is cued toward the receptive field of a recorded neuron, many changes in the neural activity have been observed ( Noudoost et al., 2010 ; Maunsell, 2015 ). A commonly reported finding is an increase in firing rates, typically of 20–30% ( Mitchell et al., 2007 ). However, the exact magnitude of the change depends on the cortical area studied, with later areas showing stronger changes ( Luck et al., 1997 ; Noudoost et al., 2010 ). Attention is also known to impact the variability of neural firing. In particular, it decreases trial-to-trial variability as measured via the Fano Factor and decreases noise correlations between pairs of neurons. Attention has even been found to impact the electrophysiological properties of neurons in a way that reduces their likelihood of firing in bursts and also decreases the height of individual action potentials ( Anderson et al., 2013 ).

In general, the changes associated with attention are believed to increase the signal-to-noise ratio of the neurons that represent the attended stimulus, however they can also impact communication between brain areas. To this end, attention's effect on neural synchrony is important. Within a visual area, attention has been shown to increase spiking coherence in the gamma band—that is at frequencies between 30 and 70 Hz ( Fries et al., 2008 ). When a group of neurons fires synchronously, their ability to influence shared downstream areas is enhanced. Furthermore, attention may also be working to directly coordinate communication across areas. Synchronous activity between two visual areas can be a sign of increased communication and attention has been shown to increase synchrony between the neurons that represent the attended stimulus in areas V1 and V4, for example ( Bosman et al., 2012 ). Control of this cross-area synchronization appears to be carried out by the pulvinar ( Saalmann et al., 2012 ).

In addition to investigating how attention impacts neurons in the visual pathways, studies have also searched for the source of top-down attention ( Noudoost et al., 2010 ; Miller and Buschman, 2014 ). The processing of bottom-up attention appears to culminate with a saliency map produced in the lateral intraparietal area (LIP). The cells here respond when salient stimuli are in their receptive field, including task-irrelevant but salient distractors. Prefrontal areas such as FEF, on the other hand, appear to house the signals needed for top-down control of spatial attention and are less responsive to distractors.

While much of the work on the neural correlates of sensory attention focuses on the cortex, subcortical areas appear to play a strong role in the control and performance benefits of attention as well. In particular, the superior colliculus assists in both covert and overt spatial attention and inactivation of this region can impair attention ( Krauzlis et al., 2013 ). And, as mentioned above, the pulvinar plays a role in attention, particularly with respect to gating effects on cortex ( Zhou et al., 2016 ).

2.2.2. Visual Feature Attention

Feature attention is another form of covert selective attention. In the study of feature attention, instead of being cued to attend to a particular location, subjects are cued on each trial to attend to a particular visual feature such as a specific color, a particular shape, or a certain orientation. The goal of the task may be to detect if the cued feature is present on the screen or readout another one of its qualities (e.g., to answer “what color is the square?” should result in attention first deployed to squares). Valid cueing about the attended feature enhances performance. For example, when attention was directed toward a particular orientation, subjects were better able to detect faint gratings of that orientation than of any other orientation ( Rossi and Paradiso, 1995 ). While the overall task (e.g., detection of an oriented grating) remains the same, the specific instructions (detection of 90° grating vs. 60° vs. 30°) will be cued on each individual trial, or possibly blockwise. Successful trial-wise cueing indicates that this form of attention can be flexibly deployed on fast timescales.

Visual search tasks are also believed to activate feature-based attention ( Figure 2 ). In these tasks, an array of stimuli appears on a screen and subjects need to indicate—frequently with an eye movement—the location of the cued stimulus. As subjects are usually allowed to make saccades throughout the task as they search for the cued stimulus, this task combines covert feature-based attention with overt attention. In fact, signals of top-down feature-based attention have been found in FEF, the area involved in saccade choice ( Zhou and Desimone, 2011 ). Because certain features can create a pop-out effect—for example, a single red shape amongst several black ones will immediately draw attention—visual search tasks also engage bottom-up attention which, depending on the task, may need to be suppressed ( Wolfe and Horowitz, 2004 ).

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2 . Visual search tasks engage many forms of visual attention. Across the top row the progression of a visual search task is shown. First, a cue indicates the target of the visual search, in this case a blue X. Then a search array appears with many non-targets. Top-down feature attention to cells that represent the color blue and the shape X will increase their firing throughout the visual field but firing will be strongest where blue or Xs actually occur. These neural response will play a role in generating a map of covert spatial attention which can be used to explore visual space before saccading. After the shift in overt attention with the first saccade, the covert attention map is remade. Finally, the target is located and successfully saccaded to. If the visual array contained a pop-out stimulus (for example a green O) it may have captured covert spatial attention in a bottom-up way and led to an additional incorrect saccade.

Neural effects of feature-based attention in the visual system are generally similar to those of spatial attention. Neurons that represent the attended feature, for example, have increased firing rates, and those that represent very different features have suppressed rates ( Treue and Trujillo, 1999 ). As opposed to spatial attention, however, feature-based attention is spatially-global. This means that when deploying attention to a particular feature the activity of the neurons that represent that feature anywhere in visual space are modulated ( Saenz et al., 2002 ). Another difference between spatial and feature attention is the question of how sources of top-down attention target the correct neurons in the visual system. The retinotopic map, wherein nearby cells represent nearby spatial locations, makes spatial targeting straightforward, but cells are not as neatly organized according to preferred visual features.

The effects of spatial and feature attention appear to be additive ( Hayden and Gallant, 2009 ). Furthermore, both feature and spatial attention are believed to create their effects by acting on the local neural circuits that implement divisive normalization in visual cortex ( Reynolds and Heeger, 2009 ). Modeling work has shown that many of the neural effects of selective attention can be captured by assuming that top-down connections provide targeted synaptic inputs to cells in these circuits ( Lindsay et al., 2019 ). However, models that rely on effects of the neuromodulator acetylcholine can also replicate neural correlates of attention ( Sajedin et al., 2019 ).

Potential sources of top-down feature-based attention have been found in prefrontal cortex where sustained activity encodes the attended feature ( Bichot et al., 2015 ; Paneri and Gregoriou, 2017 ). Inactivating the ventral prearcuate area impairs performance on search tasks. From prefrontal areas, attention signals are believed to travel in a reverse hierarchical way wherein higher visual areas send inputs to those below them ( Ahissar and Hochstein, 2000 ).

A closely related topic to feature attention is object attention. Here, attention is not deployed to an abstract feature in advance of a visual stimulus, but rather it is applied to a particular object in the visual scene ( Chen, 2012 ). The initial feedforward pass of activity through the visual hierarchy is able to pre-attentively segregate objects from their backgrounds in parallel across the visual field, provided these objects have stark and salient differences from the background. In more crowded or complex visual scenes, recurrent and serial processing is needed in order to identify different objects ( Lamme and Roelfsema, 2000 ). Serial processing involves moving limited attentional resources from one location in the image to another; it can take the form of shifts in either covert or overt spatial attention ( Buschman and Miller, 2009 ). Recurrent connections in the visual system—that is, both horizontal connections from nearby neurons in the same visual area and feedback connections from those in higher visual areas—aid in figure-ground segregation and object identification. The question of how the brain performs perceptual grouping of low-level features into a coherent object identity has been studied for nearly a century. It is believed that attention may be required for grouping, particularly for novel or complex objects ( Roelfsema and Houtkamp, 2011 ). This may be especially important in visual search tasks that require locating an object that is defined by a conjunction of several features.

Neurally, the effects of object-based attention can spread slowly through space as parts of an object are mentally traced ( Roelfsema et al., 1998 ). Switching attention to a location outside an object appears to incur a greater cost than switching to the same distance away but within the object ( Brown and Denney, 2007 ). In addition, once attention is applied to a visual object, it is believed to activate feature-based attention for the different features of that object across the visual field ( O'Craven et al., 1999 ).

Another form of attention sometimes referred to as feature attention involves attending to an entire feature dimension. An example of this is the Stroop test, wherein the names of colors are written in different colored ink and subjects either need to read the word itself or say the color of the ink. Here attention cannot be deployed to a specific feature in advance, only to the dimensions word or color. Neurally, the switch between dimensions appears to impact sensory coding in the visual stream and is controlled by frontal areas ( Liu et al., 2003 ).

2.2.3. Computational Models of Visual Attention

Visual attention, being one of the most heavily-studied topics in the neuroscience of attention, has inspired many computational models of how attention works. In general, these models synthesize various neurophysiological findings in order to help explain how the behavioral impacts of attention arise ( Heinke and Humphreys, 2005 ).

Several computational models meant to calculate saliency have been devised ( Itti and Koch, 2001 ). These models use low-level visual feature detectors—usually designed to match those in the visual system—to create an image-specific saliency map that can predict the saccade patterns of humans in response to the same image. Another approach to calculating saliency based on information theoretic first principles has also been explored and was able to account for certain visual search behaviors ( Bruce and Tsotsos, 2009 ).

Some of the behavioral and neural correlates of attention are similar whether the attention is bottom-up or top-down. In the Biased Competition Model of attention, stimuli compete against each other to dominate the neural response ( Desimone, 1998 ). Attention (bottom-up or top-down) can thus work by biasing this competition toward the stimulus that is the target of attention. While the Biased Competition Model is sometimes used simply as a “word model” to guide intuition, explicit computational instantiations of it have also been built. A hierarchical model of the visual pathway that included top-down biasing as well as local competition mediated through horizontal connections was able to replicate multiple neural effects of attention ( Deco and Rolls, 2004 ). A model embodying similar principles but using spiking neurons was also implemented ( Deco and Rolls, 2005 ).

Similar models have been constructed explicitly to deal with attribute naming tasks such as the Stroop test described above. The Selective Attention Model (SLAM), for example, has local competition in both the sensory encoding and motor output modules and can mimic known properties of response times in easier and more challenging Stroop-like tests ( Phaf et al., 1990 ).

Visual perception has been framed and modeled as a problem of Bayesian inference ( Lee and Mumford, 2003 ). Within this context, attention can help resolve uncertainty under settings where inference is more challenging, typically by modulating priors ( Rao, 2005 ). For example, in Chikkerur et al. (2010) spatial attention functions to reduce uncertainty about object identity and feature attention reduces spatial uncertainty. These principles can capture both behavioral and neural features of attention and can be implemented in a biologically-inspired neural model.

The feature similarity gain model of attention (FSGM) is a description of the neural effects of top-down attention that can be applied in both the feature and spatial domain ( Treue and Trujillo, 1999 ). It says that the way in which a neuron's response is modulated by attention depends on that neuron's tuning. Tuning is a description of how a neuron responds to different stimuli, so according to the FSGM a neuron that prefers (that is, responds strongly to), e.g., the color blue, will have its activity enhanced by top-down attention to blue. The FSGM also says attention to non-preferred stimuli will cause a decrease in firing and that, whether increased or decreased, activity is scaled multiplicatively by attention. Though not initially defined as a computational model, this form of neural modulation has since been shown through modeling to be effective at enhancing performance on challenging visual tasks ( Lindsay and Miller, 2018 ).

Other models conceptualize attention as a dynamic routing of information through a network. An implementation of this form of attention can be found in the Selective Attention for Identification Model (SAIM) ( Heinke and Humphreys, 2003 ). Here, attention routes information from the retina to a representation deemed the “focus of attention”; depending on the current task, different parts of the retinal representation will be mapped to the focus of attention.

2.2.4. Attention in Other Sensory Modalities

A famous example of the need for selective attention in audition is the “cocktail party problem”: the difficulty of focusing on the speech from one speaker in a crowded room of multiple speakers and other noises ( Bronkhorst, 2015 ). Solving the problem is believed to involve “early” selection wherein low level features of a voice such as pitch are used to determine which auditory information is passed on for further linguistic processing. Interestingly, selective auditory attention has the ability to control neural activity at even the earliest level of auditory processing, the cochlea ( Fritz et al., 2007 ).

Spatial and feature attention have also been explored in the somatosensory system. Subjects cued to expect a tap at different parts on their body are better able to detect the sensation when that cue is valid. However, these effects seem weaker than they are in the visual system ( Johansen-Berg and Lloyd, 2000 ). Reaction times are faster in a detection task when subjects are cued about the orientation of a stimulus on their finger ( Schweisfurth et al., 2014 ).

In a study that tested subjects' ability to detect a taste they had been cued for it was shown that validly-cued tastes can be detected at lower concentrations than invalidly-cued ones ( Marks and Wheeler, 1998 ). This mimics the behavioral effects found with feature-based visual attention. Attention to olfactory features has not been thoroughly explored, though visually-induced expectations about a scent can aid its detection ( Gottfried and Dolan, 2003 ; Keller, 2011 ).

Attention can also be spread across modalities to perform tasks that require integration of multiple sensory signals. In general, the use of multiple congruent sensory signals aids detection of objects when compared to relying only on a single modality. Interestingly, some studies suggest that humans may have a bias for the visual domain, even when the signal from another domain is equally valid ( Spence, 2009 ). Specifically, the visual domain appears to dominate most in tasks that require identifying the spatial location of a cue ( Bertelson and Aschersleben, 1998 ). This can be seen most readily in ventriloquism, where the visual cue of the dummy's mouth moving overrides auditory evidence about the true location of the vocal source. Visual evidence can also override tactile evidence, for example, in the context of the rubber arm illusion ( Botvinick and Cohen, 1998 ).

Another effect of the cross-modal nature of sensory processing is that an attentional cue in one modality can cause an orienting of attention in another modality ( Spence and Driver, 2004 ). Generally, the attention effects in the non-cued modality are weaker. This cross-modal interaction can occur in the context of both endogenous (“top-down”) and exogenous (“bottom-up”) attention.

2.3. Attention and Executive Control

With multiple simultaneous competing tasks, a central controller is needed to decide which to engage in and when. What's more, how to best execute tasks can depend on history and context. Combining sensory inputs with past knowledge in order to coordinate multiple systems for the job of efficient task selection and execution is the role of executive control, and this control is usually associated with the prefrontal cortex ( Miller and Buschman, 2014 ). As mentioned above, sources of top-down visual attention have also been located in prefrontal regions. Attention can reasonably be thought of as the output of executive control. The executive control system must thus select the targets of attention and communicate that to the systems responsible for implementing it. According to the reverse hierarchy theory described above, higher areas signal to those from which they get input which send the signal on to those below them and so on ( Ahissar and Hochstein, 2000 ). This means that, at each point, the instructions for attention must be transformed into a representation that makes sense for the targeted region. Through this process, the high level goals of the executive control region can lead to very specific changes, for example, in early sensory processing.

Executive control and working memory are also intertwined, as the ability to make use of past information as well as to keep a current goal in mind requires working memory. Furthermore, working memory is frequently identified as sustained activity in prefrontal areas. A consequence of the three-way relationship between executive control, working memory, and attention is that the contents of working memory can impact attention, even when not desirable for the task ( Soto et al., 2008 ). For example, if a subject has to keep an object in working memory while simultaneously performing a visual search for a separate object, the presence of the stored object in the search array can negatively interfere with the search ( Soto et al., 2005 ). This suggests that working memory can interfere with the executive control of attention. However, there still appears to be additional elements of that control that working memory alone does not disrupt. This can be seen in studies wherein visual search performance is even worse when subjects believe they will need to report the memorized item but are shown a search array for the attended item instead ( Olivers and Eimer, 2011 ). This suggests that, while all objects in working memory may have some influence over attention, the executive controller can choose which will have the most.

Beyond the flexible control of attention within a sensory modality, attention can also be shifted between modalities. Behavioral experiments indicate that switching attention either between two different tasks within a sensory modality (for example, going from locating a visual object to identifying it) or between sensory modalities (switching from an auditory task to a visual one) incurs a computational cost ( Pashler, 2000 ). This cost is usually measured as the extent to which performance is worse on trials just after the task has been switched vs. those where the same task is being repeated. Interestingly, task switching within a modality seems to incur a larger cost than switching between modalities ( Murray et al., 2009 ). A similar result is found when switching between or across modes of response (for example, pressing a bottom vs. verbal report), suggesting this is not specific to sensory processing ( Arrington et al., 2003 ). Such findings are believed to stem from the fact that switching within a modality requires a reconfiguration of the same neural circuits, which is more difficult than merely engaging the circuitry of a different sensory system. An efficient executive controller would need to be aware of these costs when deciding to shift attention and ideally try to minimize them; it has been shown that switch costs can be reduced with training ( Gopher, 1996 ).

The final question regarding the executive control of attention is how it evolves with learning. Eye movement studies indicate that searched-for items can be detected more rapidly in familiar settings rather than novel ones, suggesting that previously-learned associations guide overt attention ( Chun and Jiang, 1998 ). Such benefits are believed to rely on the hippocampus ( Aly and Turk-Browne, 2017 ). In general, however, learning how to direct attention is not as studied as other aspects of the attention process. Some studies have shown that subjects can enhance their ability to suppress irrelevant task information, and the generality of that suppression depends on the training procedure ( Kelley and Yantis, 2009 ). Looking at the neural correlates of attention learning, imaging results suggest that the neural changes associated with learning do not occur in the sensory pathways themselves but rather in areas more associated with attentional control ( Kelley and Yantis, 2010 ). Though not always easy to study, the development of attentional systems in infancy and childhood may provide further clues as to how attention can be learned ( Reynolds and Romano, 2016 ).

2.4. Attention and Memory

Attention and memory have many possible forms of interaction. If memory has a limited capacity, for example, it makes sense for the brain to be selective about what is allowed to enter it. In this way, the ability of attention to dynamically select a subset of total information is well-matched to the needs of the memory system. In the other direction, deciding to recall a specific memory is a choice about how to deploy limited resources. Therefore, both memory encoding and retrieval can rely on attention.

The role of attention in memory encoding appears quite strong ( Aly and Turk-Browne, 2017 ). For information to be properly encoded into memory, it is best for it be the target of attention. When subjects are asked to memorize a list of words while simultaneously engaging in a secondary task that divides their attention, their ability to consciously recall those words later is impaired (though their ability to recognize the words as familiar is not so affected) ( Gardiner and Parkin, 1990 ). Imaging studies have shown that increasing the difficulty of the secondary task weakens the pattern of activity related to memory encoding in the left ventral inferior frontal gyrus and anterior hippocampus and increases the representation of secondary task information in dorsolateral prefrontal and superior parietal regions ( Uncapher and Rugg, 2005 ). Therefore, without the limited neural processing power placed on the task of encoding, memory suffers. Attention has also been implicated in the encoding of spatially-defined memories and appears to stabilize the representations of place cells ( Muzzio et al., 2009 ).

Implicit statistical learning can also be biased by attention. For example, in Turk-Browne et al. (2005) subjects watched a stream of stimuli comprised of red and green shapes. The task was to detect when a shape of the attended color appeared twice in a row. Unbeknownst to the subjects, certain statistical regularities existed in the stream such that there were triplets of shapes likely to occur close together. When shown two sets of three shapes—one an actual co-occurring triplet and another a random selection of shapes of the same color—subjects recognized the real triplet as more familiar, but only if the triplets were from the attended color. The statistical regularities of the unattended shapes were not learned.

Yet some learning can occur even without conscious attention. For example, in Watanabe (2003) patients engaged in a letter detection task located centrally in their visual field while random dot motion was shown in the background at sub-threshold contrast. The motion had 10% coherence in a direction that was correlated with the currently-presented letter. Before and after learning this task, subjects performed an above-threshold direction classification task. After learning the task, direction classification improved only for the direction associated with the targeted letters. This suggests a reward-related signal activated by the target led to learning about a non-attended component of the stimulus.

Many behavioral studies have explored the extent to which attention is needed for memory retrieval. For example, by asking subjects to simultaneously recall a list of previously-memorized words and engage in a secondary task like card sorting, researchers can determine if memory retrieval pulls from the same limited pool of attentional resources as the task. Some such studies have found that retrieval is impaired by the co-occurrence of an attention-demanding task, suggesting it is an attention-dependent process. The exact findings, however, depend on the details of the memory and non-memory tasks used ( Lozito and Mulligan, 2006 ).

Even if memory retrieval does not pull from shared attentional resources, it is still clear that some memories are selected for more vivid retrieval at any given moment than others. Therefore, a selection process must occur. An examination of neuroimaging results suggests that the same parietal brain regions responsible for the top-down allocation and bottom-up capture of attention may play analogous roles during memory retrieval ( Wagner et al., 2005 ; Ciaramelli et al., 2008 ).

Studies of memory retrieval usually look at medium to long-term memory but a mechanism for attention to items in working memory has also been proposed ( Manohar et al., 2019 ). It relies on two different mechanisms of working memory: synaptic traces for non-attended items and sustained activity for the attended one.

Some forms of memory occur automatically and within the sensory processing stream itself. Priming is a well-known phenomenon in psychology wherein the presence of a stimulus at one point in time impacts how later stimuli are processed or interpreted. For example, the word “doctor” may be recognized more quickly following the word “hospital” than the word “school.” In this way, priming requires a form of implicit memory to allow previous stimuli to impact current ones. Several studies on conceptual or semantic priming indicate that attention to the first stimulus is required for priming effects to occur ( Ballesteros and Mayas, 2015 ); this mirrors findings that attention is required for memory encoding more generally.

Most priming is positive, meaning that the presence of a stimulus at one time makes the detection and processing of it or a related stimulus more likely at a later time. In this way, priming can be thought of as biasing bottom-up attention. However, top-down attention can also create negative priming. In negative priming, when stimuli that functioned as a distractor on the previous trial serve as the target of attention on the current trial, performance suffers ( Frings et al., 2015 ). This may stem from a holdover effect wherein the mechanisms of distractor suppression are still activated for the now-target stimulus.

Adaptation can also be considered a form of implicit memory. Here, neural responses decrease after repeated exposure to the same stimulus. By reducing the response to repetition, changes in the stimulus become more salient. Attention—by increasing the neural response to attended stimuli—counters the effects of adaptation ( Pestilli et al., 2007 ; Anton-Erxleben et al., 2013 ). Thus, both with priming and adaptation, top-down attention can overcome automatic processes that occur at lower levels which may be guiding bottom-up attention.

3. Attention in Machine Learning

While the concept of artificial attention has come up prior to the current resurgence of artificial neural networks, many of its popular uses today center on ANNs ( Mancas et al., 2016 ). The use of attention mechanisms in artificial neural networks came about—much like the apparent need for attention in the brain—as a means of making neural systems more flexible. Attention mechanisms in machine learning allow a single trained artificial neural network to perform well on multiple tasks or tasks with inputs of variable length, size, or structure. While the spirit of attention in machine learning is certainly inspired by psychology, its implementations do not always track with what is known about biological attention, as will be noted below.

In the form of attention originally developed for ANNs, attention mechanisms worked within an encoder-decoder framework and in the context of sequence models ( Cho et al., 2015 ; Chaudhari et al., 2019 ). Specifically, an input sequence will be passed through an encoder (likely a recurrent neural network) and the job of the decoder (also likely a recurrent neural network) will be to output another sequence. Connecting the encoder and decoder is an attention mechanism.

Commonly, the output of the encoder is a set of a vectors, one for each element in the input sequence. Attention helps determine which of these vectors should be used to generate the output. Because the output sequence is dynamically generated one element at a time, attention can dynamically highlight different encoded vectors at each time point. This allows the decoder to flexibly utilize the most relevant parts of the input sequence.

The specific job of the attention mechanism is to produce a set of scalar weightings, α t i , one for each of the encoded vectors ( v i ). At each step t , the attention mechanism (ϕ) will take in information about the decoder's previous hidden state ( h t −1 ) and the encoded vectors to produce unnormalized weightings:

Because attention is a limited resource, these weightings need to represent relative importance. To ensure that the α values sum to one, the unnormalized weightings are passed through a softmax:

These attention values scale the encoded vectors to create a single context vector on which the decoder can be conditioned:

This form of attention can be made entirely differentiable and so the whole network can be trained end-to-end with simple gradient descent.

This type of artificial attention is thus a form of iterative re-weighting. Specifically, it dynamically highlights different components of a pre-processed input as they are needed for output generation. This makes it flexible and context dependent, like biological attention. As such it is also inherently dynamic. While sequence modeling already has an implied temporal component, this form of attention can also be applied to static inputs and outputs (as will be discussed below in the context of image processing) and will thus introduce dynamics into the model.

In the traditional encoder-decoder framework without attention, the encoder produced a fixed-length vector that was independent of the length or features of the input and static during the course of decoding. This forced long sequences or sequences with complex structure to be represented with the same dimensionality as shorter or simpler ones and didn't allow the decoder to interrogate different parts of the input during the decoding process. But encoding the input as a set of vectors equal in length to the input sequence makes it possible for the decoder to selectively attend to the portion of the input sequence relevant at each time point of the decoding. Again, as in interpretations of attention in the brain, attention in artificial systems is helpful as a way to flexibly wield limited resources. The decoder can't reasonably be conditioned on the entirety of the input so at some point a bottleneck must be introduced. In the system without attention, the fixed-length encoding vector was a bottleneck. When an attention mechanism is added, the encoding can be larger because the bottleneck (in the form of the context vector) will be produced dynamically as the decoder determines which part of the input to attend to.

The motivation for adding such attention mechanisms to artificial systems is of course to improve their performance. But another claimed benefit of attention is interpretability. By identifying on which portions of the input attention is placed (that is, which α i values are high) during the decoding process, it may be possible to gain an understanding of why the decoder produced the output that it did. However, caution should be applied when interpreting the outputs of attention as they may not always explain the behavior of the model as expected ( Jain and Wallace, 2019 ; Wiegreffe and Pinter, 2019 ).

In the following subsections, specific applications of this general attention concept will be discussed, along with some that don't fit neatly into this framework. Further analogies to the biology will also be highlighted.

3.1. Attention for Natural Language Processing

As described above, attention mechanisms have frequently been added to models charged with processing sequences. Natural language processing (NLP) is one of the most common areas of application for sequence modeling. And, though it was not the original domain of attention in machine learning—nor does it have the most in common with biology—NLP is also one of the most common areas of application for attention ( Galassi et al., 2019 ).

An early application of the this form of attention in artificial neural networks was to the task of translation ( Bahdanau et al., 2014 ) ( Figure 3 ). In this work, a recurrent neural network encodes the input sentence as a set of “annotation” vectors, one for each word in the sentence. The output, a sentence in the target language, is generated one word at a time by a recurrent neural network. The probability of each generated word is a function of the previously generated word, the hidden state of the recurrent neural network and a context vector generated by the attention mechanism. Here, the attention mechanism is a small feedforward neural network that takes in the hidden state of the output network as well as the current annotation vector to create the weighting over all annotation vectors.

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3 . Attention for neural machine translation. The to-be-translated sentence is encoded to a series of vectors ( v ) via a recurrent neural network. The attention mechanism (ϕ) uses the hidden state of the decoder ( h ) and these vectors to determine how the encoded vectors should be combined to produce a context vector ( c ), which influences the next hidden state of the decoder and thus the next word in the translated sentence.

Blending information from all the words in the sentence this way allows the network to pull from earlier or later parts when generating an output word. This can be especially useful for translating between languages with different standard word orders. By visualizing the locations in the input sentence to which attention was applied the authors observed attention helping with this problem.

Since this initial application, many variants of attention networks for language translation have been developed. In Firat et al. (2016) , the attention mechanism was adapted so it could be used to translate between multiple pairs of languages rather than just one. In Luong et al. (2015) , the authors explore different structures of attention to determine if the ability to access all input words at once is necessary. And in Cheng et al. (2016) , attention mechanisms were added to the recurrent neural networks that perform the sentence encoding and decoding in order to more flexibly create sentence representations.

In 2017, the influential “Attention is All You Need” paper utilized a very different style of architecture for machine translation ( Vaswani et al., 2017 ). This model doesn't have any recurrence, making it simpler to train. Instead, words in the sentence are encoded in parallel and these encodings generate key and query representations that are combined to create attention weightings. These weightings scale the word encodings themselves to create the next layer in the model, a process known as “self-attention.” This process repeats, and eventually interacts with the autoregressive decoder which also has attention mechanisms that allow it to flexibly focus on the encoded input (as in the standard form of attention) and on the previously generated output. The Transformer—the name given to this new attention architecture—outperformed many previous models and quickly became the standard for machine translation as well as other tasks ( Devlin et al., 2018 ).

Interestingly, self-attention has less in common with biological attention than the recurrent attention models originally used for machine translation. First, it reduces the role of recurrence and dynamics, whereas the brain necessarily relies on recurrence in sequential processing tasks, including language processing and attentional selection. Second, self-attention provides a form of horizontal interaction between words—which allows for words in the encoded sentence to be processed in the context of those around them—but this mechanism does not include an obvious top-down component driven by the needs of the decoder. In fact, self-attention has been shown under certain circumstances to simply implement a convolution, a standard feedforward computation frequently used in image processing ( Andreoli, 2019 ; Cordonnier et al., 2019 ). In this way, self-attention is more about creating a good encoding than performing a task-specific attention-like selection based on limited resources. In the context of a temporal task, its closest analogue in psychology may be priming because priming alters the encoding of subsequent stimuli based on those that came before. It is of course not the direct goal of machine learning engineers to replicate the brain, but rather to create networks that can be easily trained to perform well on tasks. These different constraints mean that even large advances in machine learning do not necessarily create more brain-like models.

While the study of attention in human language processing is not as large as other areas of neuroscience research, some work has been done to track eye movements while reading ( Myachykov and Posner, 2005 ). They find that people will look back at previous sections of text in order to clarify what they are currently reading, particularly in the context of finding the antecedent of a pronoun. Such shifts in overt attention indicate what previous information is most relevant for the current processing demands.

3.2. Attention for Visual Tasks

As in neuroscience and psychology, a large portion of studies in machine learning are done on visual tasks. One of the original attention-inspired tools of computer vision is the saliency map, which identifies which regions in an image are most salient based on a set of low-level visual features such as edges, color, or depth and how they differ from their surround ( Itti and Koch, 2001 ). In this way, saliency maps indicate which regions would be captured by “bottom-up” attention in humans and animals. Computer scientists have used saliency maps as part of their image processing pipeline to identify regions for further processing.

In more recent years, computer vision models have been dominated by deep learning. And since their success in the 2012 ImageNet Challenge ( Russakovsky et al., 2015 ), convolutional neural networks have become the default architecture for visual tasks in machine learning.

The architecture of convolutional neural networks is loosely based on the mammalian visual system ( Lindsay, 2020 ). At each layer, a bank of filters is applied to the activity of the layer below (in the first layer this is the image). This creates a H × W × C tensor of neural activity with the number of channels, C equal to the number of filters applied and H and W representing the height and width of the 2-D feature maps that result from the application of a filter.

Attention in convolutional neural networks has been used to enhance performance on a variety of tasks including classification, segmentation, and image-inspired natural language processing. Also, as in the neuroscience literature, these attentional processes can be divided into spatial and feature-based attention.

3.2.1. Spatial Attention

Building off of the structures used for attention in NLP tasks, visual attention has been applied to image captioning. In Xu et al. (2015) , the encoding model is a convolutional neural network. The attention mechanism works over the activity at the fourth convolutional layer. As each word of the caption is generated, a different pattern of weighting across spatial locations of the image representation is created. In this way, attention for caption generation replaces the set of encoded word vectors in a translation task with a set of encoded image locations. Visualizing the locations with high weights, the model appears to attend to the object most relevant to the current word being generated for the caption.

This style of attention is referred to as “soft” because it produces a weighted combination of the visual features over spatial locations ( Figure 4B ). “Hard” attention is an alternative form that chooses a single spatial location to be passed into the decoder at the expense of all others ( Figure 4A ). In Xu et al. (2015) , to decide which location should receive this hard attention, the attention weights generated for each spatial location were treated as probabilities. One location is chosen according to these probabilities. Adding this stochastic element to the network makes training more difficult, yet it was found to perform somewhat better than soft attention.

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4 . Hard vs. soft visual attention in artificial neural networks. (A) In hard attention, the network only gets input from a small portion of the whole image. This portion is iteratively chosen by the network through an attention selection mechanism. If the input is foveated, the network can use the lower resolution periphery to guide this selection. (B) Feature maps in convolutional neural networks are 2-D grids of activation created by the application of a filter to the layer below. In soft spatial attention, different locations on these grids are weighted differently. In soft feature attention, different feature maps are weighted differently.

A 2014 study used reinforcement learning to train a hard attention network to perform object recognition in challenging conditions ( Mnih et al., 2014 ). The core of this model is a recurrent neural network that both keeps track of information taken in over multiple “glimpses” made by the network and outputs the location of the next glimpse. For each glimpse, the network receives a fovea-like input (central areas are represented with high resolution and peripheral with lower) from a small patch of the image. The network has to integrate the information gained from these glimpses to find and classify the object in the image. This is similar to the hard attention described above, except the selection of a location here determines which part of the image is sampled next (whereas in the case above it determined which of the already-processed image locations would be passed to the decoder). With the use of these glimpses, the network is not required to process all of the image, saving computational resources. It can also help when multiple objects are present in the image and the network must classify each ( Ba et al., 2014 ). Recent work has shown that adding a pre-training step enhances the performance of hard attention applied to complex images ( Elsayed et al., 2019 ).

In many ways, the correspondence between biological and artificial attention is strongest when it comes to visual spatial attention. For example, this form of hard attention—where different locations of the image are sequentially-sampled for further processing—replicates the process of saccading and is therefore akin to overt visual attention in the neuroscience and psychology literature. Insofar as soft attention dynamically re-weights different regions of the network's representation of the image without any change in the input to the network, it is akin to covert spatial attention. Also, as the mode of application for soft attention involves multiplicative scaling of the activity of all units at a specific location, it replicates neural findings about covert spatial attention.

Soft spatial attention has been used for other tasks, including visual question and answering ( Chen et al., 2015 ; Xu and Saenko, 2016 ; Yang et al., 2016 ) and action recognition in videos ( Sharma et al., 2015 ). Hard attention has also been used for instance segmentation ( Ren and Zemel, 2017 ) and for fine-grained classification when applied using different levels of image resolution ( Fu et al., 2017 ).

3.2.2. Feature Attention

In the case of soft spatial attention, weights are different in different spatial locations of the image representation yet they are the same across all feature channels at that location. That is, the activity of units in the network representing different visual features will all be modified the same way if they represent the same location in image space. Feature attention makes it possible to dynamically re-weight individual feature maps, creating a spatially global change in feature processing.

In Stollenga et al. (2014) , a convolutional neural network is equipped with a feature-based attention mechanism. After an image is passed through the standard feedforward architecture, the activity of the network is passed into a policy that determines how the different feature maps at different layers should be weighted. This re-weighting leads to different network activity which leads to different re-weightings. After the network has run for several timesteps the activity at the final layer is used to classify the object in the image. The policy that determines the weighting values is learned through reinforcement learning, and can be added to any pre-trained convolutional neural network.

The model in Chen et al. (2017) combines feature and spatial attention to aid in image captioning. The activity of the feedforward pass of the convolutional network is passed into the attention mechanism along with the previously generated word to create attention weightings for different channels at each layer in the CNN. These weights are used to scale activity and then a separate attention mechanism does the same procedure for generating spatial weightings. Both spatial and feature attention weights are generated and applied to the network at each time point.

In the model in De Vries et al. (2017) , the content of a question is used to control how a CNN processes an image for the task of visual question and answering. Specifically, the activity of a language embedding network is passed through a multi-layer perceptron to produce the additive and multiplicative parameters for batch normalization of each channel in the CNN. This procedure, termed conditional batch normalization, functions as a form of question-dependent feature attention.

A different form of dynamic feature re-weighting appears in “squeeze-and-excitation” networks ( Hu et al., 2018 ). In this architecture, the weightings applied to different channels are a nonlinear function of the activity of the other channels at the same layer. As with “self-attention” described above, this differs in spirit from more “top-down” approaches where weightings are a function of activity later in the network and/or biased by the needs of the output generator. Biologically speaking, this form of interaction is most similar to horizontal connections within a visual area, which are known to carry out computations such as divisive normalization ( Carandini and Heeger, 2012 ).

In the study of the biology of feature-based attention, subjects are usually cued to attend to or search for specific visual features. In this way, the to-be-attended features are known in advance and relate to the specific sub-task at hand (e.g., detection of a specific shape on a given trial of a general shape detection task). This differs from the above instances of artificial feature attention, wherein no external cue biases the network processing before knowledge about the specific image is available. Rather, the feature re-weighting is a function of the image itself and meant to enhance the performance of the network on a constant task (note this was also the case for the forms of artificial spatial attention described).

The reason for using a cueing paradigm in studies of biological attention is that it allows the experimenter to control (and thus know) where attention is placed. Yet, it is clear that even without explicit cueing, our brains make decisions about where to place attention constantly; these are likely mediated by local and long-range feedback connections to the visual system ( Wyatte et al., 2014 ). Therefore, while the task structure differs between the study of biological feature attention and its use in artificial systems, this difference may only be superficial. Essentially, the artificial systems are using feedforward image information to internally generate top-down attentional signals rather than being given the top-down information in the form of a cue.

That being said, some artificial systems do allow for externally-cued feature attention. For example setting a prior over categories in the network in Cao et al. (2015) makes it better at localizing the specific category. The network in Wang et al. (2014) , though not convolutional, has a means of biasing the detection of specific object categories as well. And in Lindsay and Miller (2018) , several performance and neural aspects of biological feature attention during a cued object detection task were replicated using a CNN. In Luo et al. (2020) , the costs and benefits of using a form of cued attention in CNNs were explored.

As mentioned above, the use of multiplicative scaling of activity is in line with certain findings from biological visual attention. Furthermore, modulating entire feature maps by the same scalar value is aligned with the finding mentioned above that feature attention acts in a spatially global way in the visual system.

3.3. Multi-Task Attention

Multi-task learning is a challenging topic in machine learning. When one network is asked to perform several different tasks—for example, a CNN that must classify objects, detect edges, and identify salient regions—training can be difficult as the weights needed to do each individual task may contradict each other. One option is have a set of task-specific parameters that modulate the activity of the shared network differently for each task. While not always called it, this can reasonably be considered a form of attention, as it flexibly alters the functioning of the network.

In Maninis et al. (2019) , a shared feedforward network is trained on all of multiple tasks, while task specific skip connections and squeeze-and-excitation blocks are trained to modulate this activity only on their specific task. This lets the network benefit from sharing processing that is common to all tasks while still specializing somewhat to each.

A similar procedure was used in Rebuffi et al. (2017) to create a network that performs classification on multiple different image domains. There, the domain could be identified from the input image making it possible to select the set of task-specific parameters automatically at run-time.

In Zhao et al. (2018) , the same image can be passed into the network and be classified along different dimensions (e.g. whether the person in the picture is smiling or not, young or old). Task-specific re-weighting of feature channels is used to execute these different classifications.

The model in Strezoski et al. (2019) uses what could be interpreted as a form of hard feature attention to route information differently in different tasks. Binary masks over feature channels are chosen randomly for each task. These masks are applied in a task-specific way during training on all tasks and at run-time. Note that in this network no task-specific attentional parameters are learned, as these masks are pre-determined and fixed during training. Instead, the network learns to use the different resulting information pathways to perform different tasks.

In a recent work, the notion of task-specific parameters was done away with entirely ( Levi and Ullman, 2020 ). Instead, the activations of a feedforward CNN are combined with a task input and passed through a second CNN to generate a full set of modulatory weights. These weights then scale the activity of the original network in a unit-specific way (thus implementing both spatial and feature attention). The result is a single set of feedforward weights capable of flexibly engaging in multiple visual tasks.

When the same input is processed differently according to many different tasks, these networks are essentially implementing a form of within-modality task switching that relies on feature attention. In this way, it is perhaps most similar to the Stroop test described previously.

3.4. Attention to Memory

Deep neural networks tend not to have explicit memory, and therefore attention to memory is not studied. Neural Turing Machines, however, are a hybrid neural architecture that includes external memory stores ( Graves et al., 2014 ). The network, through training, learns how to effectively interact with these stores to perform tasks such as sorting and repetition of stored sequences. Facilitating this interaction is a form of attention. Memories are stored as a set of vectors. To retrieve information from this store, the network generates a weight for each vector and calculates a weighted sum of the memories. To determine these weights, a recurrent neural network (which receives external and task-relevant input) outputs a vector and memories are weighted in accordance to their similarity to this vector. Thus, at each point in time, the network is able to access context-relevant memories.

As described previously, how the brain chooses what memories to attend to and then attends to them is not entirely clear. The use of a similarity metric in this model means that memories are retrieved based on their overlap with a produced activity vector, similar to associative memory models in the neuroscience literature. This offers a mechanism for the latter question—that is, how attention to memory could be implemented in the brain. The activity vector that the model produces controls what memories get attended and the relationship with biology is less clear here.

4. Ideas for Future Interaction Between Artificial and Biological Attention

As has been shown, some amount of inspiration from biology has already led to several instances of attention in artificial neural networks (summarized in Figure 5 ). While the addition of such attention mechanisms has led to appreciable increases in performance in these systems, there are clearly still many ways in which they fall short and additional opportunities for further inspiration exist. In the near term, this inspiration will likely be in the form of incremental improvements to specialized artificial systems as exist now. However, the true promise of brain-inspired AI should deliver a more integrated, multiple-purpose agent that can engage flexibly in many tasks.

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5 . An incomplete summary of the different types of attention studied in neuroscience/psychology and machine learning and how they relate. On the left are divisions of attention studied biologically, on the right are those developed for artificial intelligence and machine learning. Topics at the same horizontal location are to some extent analogous, with the distance between them indicating how close the analogy is. Forms of visual attention, for example, have the most overlap and are the most directly comparable across biology and machine learning. Some forms of attention, such as overall arousal, don't have an obvious artificial analogue.

4.1. How to Enhance Performance

There are two components to the study of how attention works in the brain that can be considered flip sides of the same coin. The first is the question of how attention enhances performance in the way that it does—that is, how do the neural changes associated with attention make the brain better at performing tasks. The second is how and why attention is deployed in the way that it is—what factors lead to the selection of certain items or tasks for attention and not others.

Neuroscientists have spent a lot of time investigating the former question. In large part, the applicability of these findings to artificial neural systems, however, may not be straightforward. Multiplicative scaling of activity appears in both biological and artificial systems and is an effective means of implementing attention. However, many of the observed effects of attention in the brain make sense mainly as a means of increasing the signal carried by noisy, spiking neurons. This includes increased synchronization across neurons and decreased firing variability. Without analogs for these changes in deep neural networks, it is hard to take inspiration from them. What's more, the training procedures for neural networks can automatically determine the changes in activity needed to enhance performance on a well-defined task and so lessons from biological changes may not be as relevant.

On the other hand, the observation that attention can impact spiking-specific features such as action potential height, burstiness, and precise spike times may indicate the usefulness of spiking networks. Specifically, spiking models offer more degrees of freedom for attention to control and thus allow attention to possibly have larger and/or more nuanced impacts.

Looking at the anatomy of attention may provide usable insights to people designing architectures for artificial systems. For example, visual attention appears to modulate activity more strongly in later visual areas like V4 ( Noudoost et al., 2010 ), whereas auditory attention can modulate activity much earlier in the processing stream. The level at which attention should act could thus be a relevant architectural variable. In this vein, recent work has shown that removing self-attention from the early layers of a Transformer model enhances its performance on certain natural language processing tasks and also makes the model a better predictor of human fMRI signals during language processing ( Toneva and Wehbe, 2019 ).

The existence of cross-modal cueing—wherein attention cued in one sensory modality can cause attention to be deployed to the same object or location in another modality—indicates some amount of direct interaction between different sensory systems. Whereas many multi-modal models in machine learning use entirely separate processing streams that are only combined at the end, allowing some horizontal connections between different input streams may help coordinate their processing.

Attention also interacts with the kind of adaptation that normally occurs in sensory processing. Generally, neural network models do not have mechanisms for adaptation—that is, neurons have no means of reducing their activity if given the same input for multiple time steps. Given that adaptation helps make changes and anomalies stand out, it may be useful to include. In a model with adaption, attention mechanisms should work to reactivate adapted neurons if the repeated stimulus is deemed important.

Finally, some forms of attention appear to act in multiple ways on the same system. For example, visual attention is believed to both: (1) enhance the sensitivity of visual neurons in the cortex by modulating their activity and (2) change subcortical activity such that sensory information is readout differently ( Birman and Gardner, 2019 ; Sreenivasan and Sridharan, 2019 ). In this way, attention uses two different mechanisms, in different parts of the brain, to create its effect. Allowing attention to modulate multiple components of a model architecture in complementary ways may allow it to have more robust and effective impacts.

4.2. How to Deploy Attention

The question of how to deploy attention is likely the more relevant challenge for producing complex and integrated artificial intelligence. Choosing the relevant information in a stream of incoming stimuli, picking the best task to engage in, or deciding whether to engage in anything at all requires that an agent have an integrative understanding of its state, environment, and needs.

The most direct way to take influence from biological attention is to mimic it directly. Scanpath models, for example, have existed in the study of saliency for many years. They attempt to predict the series of fixations that humans make while viewing images ( Borji and Itti, 2019 ). A more direct approach to training attention was used in Linsley et al. (2018) . Here, a large dataset of human top-down attention was collected by having subjects label the regions of images most relevant for object classification. The task-specific saliency maps created through this method were used to train attention in a deep convolutional neural network whose main task was object recognition. They found that influencing the activity of intermediate layers with this method could increase performance. Another way of learning a teacher's saliency map was given in Zagoruyko and Komodakis (2016) .

Combined training on tasks and neural data collected from human visual areas has also helped the performance of CNNs ( Fong et al., 2018 ). Using neural data collected during attention tasks in particular could help train attention models. Such transfer could also be done for other tasks. For example, tracking eye movements during reading could inform NLP models; thus far, eye movements have been used to help train a part-of-speech tagging model ( Barrett et al., 2016 ). Interestingly, infants may learn from attending to what adults around them attend to and the coordination of attention more broadly across agents may be very helpful in a social species. Therefore, the attention of others should influence how attention is guided. Attempts to coordinate joint attention will need to be integrated into attention systems ( Kaplan and Hafner, 2006 ; Klein et al., 2009 ).

Activities would likely need to flexibly decide which of several possible goals should be achieved at any time and therefore where attention should be placed. This problem clearly interacts closely with issues around reinforcement learning—particularly hierarchical reinforcement learning which involves the choosing of subtasks—as such decisions must be based on expected positive or negative outcomes. Indeed, there is a close relationship between attention and reward as previously rewarded stimuli attract attention even in contexts where they no longer provide reward ( Camara et al., 2013 ). A better understanding of how humans choose which tasks to engage in and when should allow human behavior to inform the design of a multi-task AI.

To this end, the theory put forth in Shenhav et al. (2013) , which says that allocation of the brain's limited ability to control different processes is based on the expected value of that control, may be of use. In this framework, the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex is responsible for integrating diverse information—including the cognitive costs of control—in order to calculate the expected value of control and thus direct processes like attention. Another approach for understanding human executive control in complex tasks is inverse reinforcement learning. This method was recently applied to a dataset of eye movements during visual search in order to determine the reward functions and policies used by humans ( Zelinsky et al., 2020 ).

An additional factor that drives biological attention but is perhaps underrepresented in artificial attention systems is curiosity ( Gottlieb et al., 2013 ). In biology, novel, confusing, and surprising stimuli can grab attention, and inferotemporal and perirhinal cortex are believed to signal novel visual situations via an adaptation mechanism that reduces responses to familiar inputs. Reinforcement learning algorithms that include novelty as part of the estimate of the value of a state can encourage this kind of exploration ( Jaegle et al., 2019 ). How exactly to calculate surprise or novelty in different circumstances is not always clear, however. Previous work on biological attention has understood attention selection in Bayesian terms of surprise or information gathering and these framings may be useful for artificial systems ( Itti and Baldi, 2006 ; Mirza et al., 2019 ).

A final issue in the selection of attention is how conflicts are resolved. Given the brain's multiple forms of attention—arousal, bottom-up, top-down, etc.—how do conflicts regarding the appropriate locus of attention get settled? Looking at the visual system, it seems that the local circuits that these multiple systems target are burdened with this task. These circuits receive neuromodulatory input along with top-down signals which they must integrate with the bottom-up input driving their activity. Horizontal connections mediate this competition, potentially using winner-take-all mechanisms. This can be mimicked in the architecture of artificial systems.

4.3. Attention and Learning

Attention, through its role in determining what enters memory, guides learning. Most artificial systems with attention include the attention mechanism throughout training. In this way, the attention mechanism is trained along with the base architecture; however, with the exception of the Neural Turing Machine, the model does not continue learning once the functioning attention system is in place. Therefore, the ability of attention to control learning and memory is still not explicitly considered in these systems.

Attention could help make efficient use of data by directing learning to the relevant components and relationships in the input. For example, saliency maps have been used as part of the pre-processing for various computer vision tasks ( Lee et al., 2004 ; Wolf et al., 2007 ; Bai and Wang, 2014 ). Focusing subsequent processing only on regions that are intrinsically salient can prevent wasteful processing on irrelevant regions and, in the context of network training, could also prevent overfitting to these regions. Using saliency maps in this way, however, requires a definition of saliency that works for the problem at hand. Using the features of images that capture bottom-up attention in humans has worked for some computer vision problems; looking at human data in other modalities may be useful as well.

In a related vein, studies on infants suggest that they have priors that guide their attention to relevant stimuli such as faces. Using such priors could bootstrap learning both of how to process important stimuli and how to better attend to their relevant features ( Johnson, 2001 ).

In addition to deciding which portions of the data to process, top-down attention can also be thought of as selecting which elements of the network should be most engaged during processing. Insofar as learning will occur most strongly in the parts of the network that are most engaged, this is another means by which attention guides learning. Constraining the number of parameters that will be updated in response to any given input is an effective form of regularization, as can be seen in the use of dropout and batch normalization. Attention—rather than randomly choosing which units to engage and disengage—is constrained to choose units that will also help performance on this task. It is therefore a more task-specific form of regularization.

In this way, attention may be particularly helpful for continual learning where the aim is to update a network to perform better on a specific task while not disrupting performance on the other tasks the network has already learned to do. A related concept, conditional computation, has recently been applied to the problem of continual learning ( Lin et al., 2019 ). In conditional computation, the parameters of a network are a function of the current input (it can thus be thought of as an extreme form of the type of modulation done by attention); optimizing the network for efficient continual learning involves controlling the amount of interference between different inputs. More generically, it may be helpful to think of attention, in part, as a means of guarding against undesirable synaptic changes.

Attention and learning also work in a loop. Specifically, attention guides what is learned about the world and internal world models are used to guide attention. This inter-dependency has recently been formalized in terms of a reinforcement learning framework that also incorporates cognitive Bayesian inference models that have succeeded in explaining human learning and decision making ( Radulescu et al., 2019 ). Interconnections between basal ganglia and prefrontal cortex are believed to support the interplay between reinforcement learning and attention selection.

At a more abstract level, the mere presence of attention in the brain's architecture can influence representation learning. The global workspace theory of consciousness says that at any moment a limited amount of information selected from the brain's activity can enter working memory and be available for further joint processing ( Baars, 2005 ). Inspired by this, the ‘consciousness prior' in machine learning emphasizes a neural network architecture with a low-dimensional representation that arises from attention applied to an underlying high-dimensional state representation ( Bengio, 2017 ). This low-D representation should efficiently represent the world at an abstract level such that it can be used to summarize and make predictions about future states. The presence of this attention-mediated bottleneck has a trickle-down effect that encourages disentangled representations at all levels such that they can be flexibly combined to guide actions and make predictions.

Conscious attention is required for the learning of many complex skills such as playing a musical instrument. However once fully learned, these processes can become automatic, possibly freeing attention up to focus on other things ( Treisman et al., 1992 ). The mechanisms of this transformation are not entirely clear but insofar as they seem to rely on moving the burden of the task to different, possibly lower/more reflexive brain areas, it may benefit artificial systems to have multiple redundant pathways that can be engaged differently by attention ( Poldrack et al., 2005 ).

4.4. Limitations of Attention: Bugs or Features?

Biological attention does not work perfectly. As mentioned above, performance can suffer when switching between different kinds of attention, arousal levels need be just right in order to reach peak performance, and top-down attention can be interrupted by irrelevant but salient stimuli. A question when transferring attention to artificial systems is are these limitations bugs to be avoided or features to be incorporated?

Distractability, in general, seems like a feature of attention rather than a bug. Even when attempting to focus on a task it is beneficial to still be aware of—and distractable by—potentially life-threatening changes in the environment. The problem comes only when an agent is overly distractable to inputs that do not pose a threat or provide relevant information. Thus, artificial systems should balance the strength of top down attention such that it still allows for the processing of unexpected but informative stimuli. For example, attentional blink refers to the phenomenon wherein a subject misses a second target in a stream of targets and distractors if it occurs quickly after a first target ( Shapiro et al., 1997 ). While this makes performance worse, it may be necessary to give the brain time to process and act on the first target. In this way, it prevents distractability to ensure follow through.

Any agent, artificial or biological, will have some limitations on its energy resources. Therefore, prudent decisions about when to engage in the world versus enter an energy-saving state such as sleep will always be of relevance. For many animals sleep occurs according to a schedule but, as was discussed, it can also be delayed or interrupted by attention-demanding situations. The decision about when to enter a sleep state must thus be made based on a cost-benefit analysis of what can be gained by staying awake. Because sleep is also known to consolidate memories and perform other vital tasks beyond just energy conservation, this decision may be a complex one. Artificial systems will need to have an integrative understanding of their current state and future demands to make this decision.

5. Conclusions

Attention is a large and complex topic that sprawls across psychology, neuroscience, and artificial intelligence. While many of the topics studied under this name are non-overlapping in their mechanisms, they do share a core theme of the flexible control of limited resources. General findings about flexibility and wise uses of resources can help guide the development of AI, as can specific findings about the best means of deploying attention to specific sensory modalities or tasks.

Author Contributions

GL conceived and wrote the article and generated the figures.

This work was supported by a Marie Skłodowska-Curie Individual Fellowship (No. 844003) and a Sainsbury Wellcome Centre/Gatsby Computational Unit Fellowship.

Conflict of Interest

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The reviewer MR declared a past co-authorship with the author GL to the handling Editor.

Acknowledgments

The author would like to thank Jacqueline Gottlieb and the three reviewers for their insights and pointers to references.

Ahissar, M., and Hochstein, S. (2000). The spread of attention and learning in feature search: effects of target distribution and task difficulty. Vis. Res. 40, 1349–1364. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(00)00002-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aly, M., and Turk-Browne, N. B. (2017). “How hippocampal memory shapes, and is shaped by, attention,” in The Hippocampus From Cells to Systems , eds D. E. Hannula and M. C. Duff (Cham: Springer), 369–403.

Google Scholar

Anderson, E. B., Mitchell, J. F., and Reynolds, J. H. (2013). Attention-dependent reductions in burstiness and action-potential height in macaque area V4. Nat. Neurosci. 16, 1125–1131. doi: 10.1038/nn.3463

Andreoli, J.-M. (2019). Convolution, attention and structure embedding. arXiv [preprint] . arXiv: 1905.01289.

Anton-Erxleben, K., and Carrasco, M. (2013). Attentional enhancement of spatial resolution: linking behavioural and neurophysiological evidence. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 14, 188–200. doi: 10.1038/nrn3443

Anton-Erxleben, K., Herrmann, K., and Carrasco, M. (2013). Independent effects of adaptation and attention on perceived speed. Psychol. Sci. 24, 150–159. doi: 10.1177/0956797612449178

Arrington, C. M., Altmann, E. M., and Carr, T. H. (2003). Tasks of a feather flock together: Similarity effects in task switching. Mem. Cogn. 31, 781–789. doi: 10.3758/BF03196116

Ba, J., Mnih, V., and Kavukcuoglu, K. (2014). Multiple object recognition with visual attention. arXiv [preprint] . arXiv:1412.7755.

Baars, B. J. (2005). Global workspace theory of consciousness: toward a cognitive neuroscience of human experience. Prog. Brain Res. 150, 45–53. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(05)50004-9

Bahdanau, D., Cho, K., and Bengio, Y. (2014). Neural machine translation by jointly learning to align and translate. arXiv [preprint] . arXiv:1409.0473.

Bai, X., and Wang, W. (2014). Saliency-SVM: an automatic approach for image segmentation. Neurocomputing 136, 243–255. doi: 10.1016/j.neucom.2014.01.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ballesteros, S., and Mayas, J. (2015). Selective attention affects conceptual object priming and recognition: a study with young and older adults. Front. Psychol. 5:1567. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01567

Barrett, M., Bingel, J., Keller, F., and Søgaard, A. (2016). “Weakly supervised part-of-speech tagging using eye-tracking data,” in Proceedings of the 54th Annual Meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics (Volume 2: Short Papers) (Berlin), 579–584.

Bengio, Y. (2017). The consciousness prior. arXiv [preprint] . arXiv:1709.08568.

Berger, A., Henik, A., and Rafal, R. (2005). Competition between endogenous and exogenous orienting of visual attention. J. Exp. Psychol. 134, 207–221. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.134.2.207

Bertelson, P., and Aschersleben, G. (1998). Automatic visual bias of perceived auditory location. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 5, 482–489. doi: 10.3758/BF03208826

Bichot, N. P., Heard, M. T., DeGennaro, E. M., and Desimone, R. (2015). A source for feature-based attention in the prefrontal cortex. Neuron 88, 832–844. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.10.001

Birman, D., and Gardner, J. L. (2019). A flexible readout mechanism of human sensory representations. Nat. Commun. 10, 1–13. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-11448-7

Borji, A., and Itti, L. (2012). State-of-the-art in visual attention modeling. IEEE Trans. Pattern Anal. Mach. Intell. 35, 185–207. doi: 10.1109/TPAMI.2012.89

Borji, A., and Itti, L. (2019). Cat2000: a large scale fixation dataset for boosting saliency research. arXiv [preprint] . arXiv:1505.03581.

Bosman, C. A., Schoffelen, J.-M., Brunet, N., Oostenveld, R., Bastos, A. M., Womelsdorf, T., et al. (2012). Attentional stimulus selection through selective synchronization between monkey visual areas. Neuron 75, 875–888. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.06.037

Botvinick, M., and Cohen, J. (1998). Rubber hands ‘feel' touch that eyes see. Nature 391, 756–756. doi: 10.1038/35784

Bronkhorst, A. W. (2015). The cocktail-party problem revisited: early processing and selection of multi-talker speech. Attent. Percept. Psychophys. 77, 1465–1487. doi: 10.3758/s13414-015-0882-9

Brown, J. M., and Denney, H. I. (2007). Shifting attention into and out of objects: evaluating the processes underlying the object advantage. Percept. Psychophys. 69, 606–618. doi: 10.3758/BF03193918

Bruce, N. D., and Tsotsos, J. K. (2009). Saliency, attention, and visual search: an information theoretic approach. J. Vis. 9:5. doi: 10.1167/9.3.5

Buschman, T. J., and Miller, E. K. (2009). Serial, covert shifts of attention during visual search are reflected by the frontal eye fields and correlated with population oscillations. Neuron 63, 386–396. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.06.020

Camara, E., Manohar, S., and Husain, M. (2013). Past rewards capture spatial attention and action choices. Exp. Brain Res. 230, 291–300. doi: 10.1007/s00221-013-3654-6

Cao, C., Liu, X., Yang, Y., Yu, Y., Wang, J., Wang, Z., et al. (2015). “Look and think twice: Capturing top-down visual attention with feedback convolutional neural networks,” in Proceedings of the IEEE International Conference on Computer Vision (Santiago, CA), 2956–2964.

Carandini, M., and Heeger, D. J. (2012). Normalization as a canonical neural computation. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 51–62. doi: 10.1038/nrn3136

Chaudhari, S., Polatkan, G., Ramanath, R., and Mithal, V. (2019). An attentive survey of attention models. arXiv [preprint] . arXiv:1904.02874.

Chen, K., Wang, J., Chen, L.-C., Gao, H., Xu, W., and Nevatia, R. (2015). ABC-CNN: an attention based convolutional neural network for visual question answering. arXiv [preprint] . arXiv:1511.05960.

Chen, L., Zhang, H., Xiao, J., Nie, L., Shao, J., Liu, W., et al. (2017). “SCA-CNN: Spatial and channel-wise attention in convolutional networks for image captioning,” in Proceedings of the IEEE Conference on Computer Vision and Pattern Recognition (Honolulu, HI), 5659–5667. doi: 10.1109/CVPR.2017.667

Chen, Z. (2012). Object-based attention: a tutorial review. Attent. Percept. Psychophys. 74, 784–802. doi: 10.3758/s13414-012-0322-z

Cheng, J., Dong, L., and Lapata, M. (2016). Long short-term memory-networks for machine reading. arXiv preprint arXiv:1601.06733 . doi: 10.18653/v1/D16-1053

Chikkerur, S., Serre, T., Tan, C., and Poggio, T. (2010). What and where: a bayesian inference theory of attention. Vis. Res. 50, 2233–2247. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2010.05.013

Cho, K., Courville, A., and Bengio, Y. (2015). Describing multimedia content using attention-based encoder-decoder networks. IEEE Trans. Multimed. 17, 1875–1886. doi: 10.1109/TMM.2015.2477044

Chun, M. M., Golomb, J. D., and Turk-Browne, N. B. (2011). A taxonomy of external and internal attention. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 62, 73–101. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100427

Chun, M. M., and Jiang, Y. (1998). Contextual cueing: implicit learning and memory of visual context guides spatial attention. Cogn. Psychol. 36, 28–71. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1998.0681

Ciaramelli, E., Grady, C. L., and Moscovitch, M. (2008). Top-down and bottom-up attention to memory: a hypothesis (atom) on the role of the posterior parietal cortex in memory retrieval. Neuropsychologia 46, 1828–1851. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.03.022

Coenen, A. M. (1998). Neuronal phenomena associated with vigilance and consciousness: from cellular mechanisms to electroencephalographic patterns. Conscious. Cogn. 7, 42–53. doi: 10.1006/ccog.1997.0324

Cordonnier, J.-B., Loukas, A., and Jaggi, M. (2019). On the relationship between self-attention and convolutional layers. arXiv [preprint] . arXiv:1911.03584.

De Vries, H., Strub, F., Mary, J., Larochelle, H., Pietquin, O., and Courville, A. C. (2017). “Modulating early visual processing by language,” in Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems (Long Beach, CA), 6594–6604.

Deco, G., and Rolls, E. T. (2004). A neurodynamical cortical model of visual attention and invariant object recognition. Vis. Res. 44, 621–642. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2003.09.037

Deco, G., and Rolls, E. T. (2005). Neurodynamics of biased competition and cooperation for attention: a model with spiking neurons. J. Neurophysiol. 94, 295–313. doi: 10.1152/jn.01095.2004

Desimone, R. (1998). Visual attention mediated by biased competition in extrastriate visual cortex. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B Biol. Sci. 353, 1245–1255. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1998.0280

Devlin, J., Chang, M.-W., Lee, K., and Toutanova, K. (2018). BERT: pre-training of deep bidirectional transformers for language understanding. arXiv [preprint] arXiv:1810.04805.

Diamond, D. M. (2005). Cognitive, endocrine and mechanistic perspectives on non-linear relationships between arousal and brain function. Nonlinearity Biolo Toxicol Med. 3, 1–7. doi: 10.2201/nonlin.003.01.001

Driver, J. (2001). A selective review of selective attention research from the past century. Br. J. Psychol. 92, 53–78. doi: 10.1348/000712601162103

Elsayed, G., Kornblith, S., and Le, Q. V. (2019). “Saccader: improving accuracy of hard attention models for vision,” in Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems (Vancouver, BC), 700–712.

Firat, O., Cho, K., and Bengio, Y. (2016). Multi-way, multilingual neural machine translation with a shared attention mechanism. arXiv preprint arXiv:1601.01073 . doi: 10.18653/v1/N16-1101

Fong, R. C., Scheirer, W. J., and Cox, D. D. (2018). Using human brain activity to guide machine learning. Sci. Rep. 8:5397. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-23618-6

Fries, P., Womelsdorf, T., Oostenveld, R., and Desimone, R. (2008). The effects of visual stimulation and selective visual attention on rhythmic neuronal synchronization in macaque area v4. J. Neurosci. 28, 4823–4835. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4499-07.2008

Frings, C., Schneider, K. K., and Fox, E. (2015). The negative priming paradigm: an update and implications for selective attention. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 22, 1577–1597. doi: 10.3758/s13423-015-0841-4

Fritz, J. B., Elhilali, M., David, S. V., and Shamma, S. A. (2007). Auditory attention–focusing the searchlight on sound. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 17, 437–455. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2007.07.011

Fu, J., Zheng, H., and Mei, T. (2017). “Look closer to see better: recurrent attention convolutional neural network for fine-grained image recognition,” in Proceedings of the IEEE Conference on Computer Vision and Pattern Recognition (Honolulu, HI), 4438–4446.

Galassi, A., Lippi, M., and Torroni, P. (2019). Attention, please! a critical review of neural attention models in natural language processing. arXiv [preprint] . arXiv:1902.02181.

Gardiner, J. M., and Parkin, A. J. (1990). Attention and recollective experience in recognition memory. Mem. Cogn. 18, 579–583.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Gopher, D. (1996). Attention control: explorations of the work of an executive controller. Cogn. Brain Res. 5, 23–38.

Gottfried, J. A., and Dolan, R. J. (2003). The nose smells what the eye sees: crossmodal visual facilitation of human olfactory perception. Neuron 39, 375–386. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00392-1

Gottlieb, J., Oudeyer, P.-Y., Lopes, M., and Baranes, A. (2013). Information-seeking, curiosity, and attention: computational and neural mechanisms. Trends Cogn. Sci. 17, 585–593. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.09.001

Graves, A., Wayne, G., and Danihelka, I. (2014). Neural turing machines. arXiv [preprint] . arXiv:1410.5401.

Hayden, B. Y., and Gallant, J. L. (2009). Combined effects of spatial and feature-based attention on responses of v4 neurons. Vis. Res. 49, 1182–1187. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2008.06.011

Hayhoe, M., and Ballard, D. (2005). Eye movements in natural behavior. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 188–194. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.02.009

Heinke, D., and Humphreys, G. W. (2003). Attention, spatial representation, and visual neglect: simulating emergent attention and spatial memory in the selective attention for identification model (SAIM). Psychol. Rev. 110, 29–87. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.110.1.29

Heinke, D., and Humphreys, G. W. (2005). Computational models of visual selective attention: a review. Connect. Models Cogn. Psychol. 1, 273–312. doi: 10.4324/9780203647110

Hommel, B., Chapman, C. S., Cisek, P., Neyedli, H. F., Song, J.-H., and Welsh, T. N. (2019). No one knows what attention is. Attent. Percept. Psychophys. 81, 2288–2303. doi: 10.3758/s13414-019-01846-w

Hu, J., Shen, L., and Sun, G. (2018). “Squeeze-and-excitation networks,” in Proceedings of the IEEE Conference on Computer Vision and Pattern Recognition (Salt Lake City, UT), 7132–7141.

Hutmacher, F. (2019). Why is there so much more research on vision than on any other sensory modality? Front. Psychol. 10:2246. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02246

Itti, L., and Baldi, P. F. (2006). “Bayesian surprise attracts human attention,” in Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems (Vancouver, BC), 547–554.

Itti, L., and Koch, C. (2001). Computational modelling of visual attention. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2, 194–203. doi: 10.1038/35058500

Jaegle, A., Mehrpour, V., and Rust, N. (2019). Visual novelty, curiosity, and intrinsic reward in machine learning and the brain. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 58, 167–174. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2019.08.004

Jain, S., and Wallace, B. C. (2019). Attention is not explanation. arXiv [preprint] . arXiv:1902.10186.

Johansen-Berg, H., and Lloyd, D. M. (2000). The physiology and psychology of selective attention to touch. Front. Biosci. 5, D894–D904. doi: 10.2741/A558

Johnson, M. H. (2001). Functional brain development in humans. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2, 475–483. doi: 10.1038/35081509

Kanwisher, N., and Wojciulik, E. (2000). Visual attention: insights from brain imaging. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 1, 91–100. doi: 10.1038/35039043

Kaplan, F., and Hafner, V. V. (2006). The challenges of joint attention. Interact. Stud. 7, 135–169. doi: 10.1075/is.7.2.04kap

Keller, A. (2011). Attention and olfactory consciousness. Front. Psychol. 2:380. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00380

Kelley, T. A., and Yantis, S. (2009). Learning to attend: effects of practice on information selection. J. Vis. 9:16. doi: 10.1167/9.7.16

Kelley, T. A., and Yantis, S. (2010). Neural correlates of learning to attend. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 4:216. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00216

Klein, J. T., Shepherd, S. V., and Platt, M. L. (2009). Social attention and the brain. Curr. Biol. 19, R958–R962. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.08.010

Krauzlis, R. J., Lovejoy, L. P., and Zénon, A. (2013). Superior colliculus and visual spatial attention. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 36, 165–182. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062012-170249

Lamme, V. A., and Roelfsema, P. R. (2000). The distinct modes of vision offered by feedforward and recurrent processing. Trends Neurosci. 23, 571–579. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(00)01657-X

Lee, S.-H., Shin, J.-K., and Lee, M. (2004). “Non-uniform image compression using biologically motivated saliency map model,” in Proceedings of the 2004 Intelligent Sensors, Sensor Networks and Information Processing Conference, 2004 (Melbourne, VIC), 525–530.

Lee, T. S., and Mumford, D. (2003). Hierarchical bayesian inference in the visual cortex. JOSA A 20, 1434–1448. doi: 10.1364/JOSAA.20.001434

Levi, H., and Ullman, S. (2020). Multi-task learning by a top-down control network. arXiv [Preprint] . arXiv:2002.03335.

Lin, M., Fu, J., and Bengio, Y. (2019). Conditional computation for continual learning. arXiv [preprint] . arXiv:1906.06635.

Lindsay, G. (2020). Convolutional neural networks as a model of the visual system: past, present, and future. J. Cogn. Neurosci. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01544. [Epub ahead of print].

Lindsay, G. W., and Miller, K. D. (2018). How biological attention mechanisms improve task performance in a large-scale visual system model. eLife 7:e38105. doi: 10.7554/eLife.38105

Lindsay, G. W., Rubin, D. B., and Miller, K. D. (2019). A simple circuit model of visual cortex explains neural and behavioral aspects of attention. bioRxiv. [preprint]. doi: 10.1101/2019.12.13.875534

Linsley, D., Shiebler, D., Eberhardt, S., and Serre, T. (2018). Learning what and where to attend. arXiv [preprint] . arXiv:1805.08819.

Liu, T., Slotnick, S. D., Serences, J. T., and Yantis, S. (2003). Cortical mechanisms of feature-based attentional control. Cereb. Cortex 13, 1334–1343. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhg080

Lozito, J. P., and Mulligan, N. W. (2006). Exploring the role of attention during memory retrieval: effects of semantic encoding and divided attention. Mem. Cogn. 34, 986–998. doi: 10.3758/BF03193246

Luck, S. J., Chelazzi, L., Hillyard, S. A., and Desimone, R. (1997). Neural mechanisms of spatial selective attention in areas V1, V2, and V4 of macaque visual cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 77, 24–42. doi: 10.1152/jn.1997.77.1.24

Luo, X., Roads, B. D., and Love, B. C. (2020). The costs and benefits of goal-directed attention in deep convolutional neural networks. arXiv [preprint] . arXiv:2002.02342.

Luong, M.-T., Pham, H., and Manning, C. D. (2015). Effective approaches to attention-based neural machine translation. arXiv preprint] . arXiv:1508.04025.

Makeig, S., Jung, T.-P., and Sejnowski, T. J. (2000). Awareness during drowsiness: dynamics and electrophysiological correlates. Can. J. Exp. Psychol. 54, 266–273. doi: 10.1037/h0087346

Mancas, M., Ferrera, V. P., Riche, N., and Taylor, J. G. (2016). From Human Attention to Computational Attention, Vol. 2 . New York, NY: Springer.

Maninis, K.-K., Radosavovic, I., and Kokkinos, I. (2019). “Attentive single-tasking of multiple tasks,” in Proceedings of the IEEE Conference on Computer Vision and Pattern Recognition (Long Beach, CA), 1851–1860. doi: 10.1109/CVPR.2019.00195

Manohar, S. G., Zokaei, N., Fallon, S. J., Vogels, T., and Husain, M. (2019). Neural mechanisms of attending to items in working memory. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev . 101, 1–12. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.03.017

Marks, L. E., and Wheeler, M. E. (1998). Attention and the detectability of weak taste stimuli. Chem. Senses 23, 19–29. doi: 10.1093/chemse/23.1.19

Maunsell, J. H. (2015). Neuronal mechanisms of visual attention. Annu. Rev. Vis. Sci. 1, 373–391. doi: 10.1146/annurev-vision-082114-035431

Miller, E. K., and Buschman, T. J. (2014). “Neural mechanisms for the executive control of attention,” in The Oxford Handbook of Attention , eds A. C. Nobre and S. Kastner (Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press).

Mirza, M. B., Adams, R. A., Friston, K., and Parr, T. (2019). Introducing a bayesian model of selective attention based on active inference. Sci. Rep. 9:13915. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-50138-8

Mitchell, J. F., Sundberg, K. A., and Reynolds, J. H. (2007). Differential attention-dependent response modulation across cell classes in macaque visual area v4. Neuron 55, 131–141. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.06.018

Mnih, V., Heess, N., Graves, A., et al. (2014). “Recurrent models of visual attention,” in Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems (Montreal, QC), 2204–2212.

Moore, T., Armstrong, K. M., and Fallah, M. (2003). Visuomotor origins of covert spatial attention. Neuron 40, 671–683. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00716-5

Murray, M. M., De Santis, L., Thut, G., and Wylie, G. R. (2009). The costs of crossing paths and switching tasks between audition and vision. Brain Cogn. 69, 47–55. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2008.05.004

Muzzio, I. A., Kentros, C., and Kandel, E. (2009). What is remembered? Role of attention on the encoding and retrieval of hippocampal representations. J. Physiol. 587, 2837–2854. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2009.172445

Myachykov, A., and Posner, M. I. (2005). “Attention in language,” in Neurobiology of Attention , eds L. Itti, G. Rees, and J. K. Tsotsos (Burlington, MA: Elsevier), 324–329.

Noudoost, B., Chang, M. H., Steinmetz, N. A., and Moore, T. (2010). Top-down control of visual attention. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 20, 183–190. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2010.02.003

O'Craven, K. M., Downing, P. E., and Kanwisher, N. (1999). fMRI evidence for objects as the units of attentional selection. Nature 401, 584–587. doi: 10.1038/44134

Oken, B. S., Salinsky, M. C., and Elsas, S. (2006). Vigilance, alertness, or sustained attention: physiological basis and measurement. Clin. Neurophysiol. 117, 1885–1901. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2006.01.017

Olivers, C. N., and Eimer, M. (2011). On the difference between working memory and attentional set. Neuropsychologia 49, 1553–1558. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.11.033

Paneri, S., and Gregoriou, G. G. (2017). Top-down control of visual attention by the prefrontal cortex. Functional specialization and long-range interactions. Front. Neurosci. 11:545. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2017.00545

Pashler, H. (2000). “Task switching and multitask performance,” in Control of Cognitive Processes: Attention and Performance XVIII , eds S. Monsell and J. Driver (MIT Press), 277. doi: 10.1002/acp.849

Pestilli, F., Viera, G., and Carrasco, M. (2007). How do attention and adaptation affect contrast sensitivity? J. Vis. 7, 9.1–9.12. doi: 10.1167/7.7.9

Phaf, R. H., Van der Heijden, A., and Hudson, P. T. (1990). SLAM: a connectionist model for attention in visual selection tasks. Cogn. Psychol. 22, 273–341. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(90)90006-P

Poldrack, R. A., Sabb, F. W., Foerde, K., Tom, S. M., Asarnow, R. F., Bookheimer, S. Y., et al. (2005). The neural correlates of motor skill automaticity. J. Neurosci. 25, 5356–5364. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3880-04.2005

Posner, M. I. (2008). Measuring alertness. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1129, 193–199. doi: 10.1196/annals.1417.011

Radulescu, A., Niv, Y., and Ballard, I. (2019). Holistic reinforcement learning: the role of structure and attention. Trends Cogn. Sci . 23, 278–292. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2019.01.010

Rao, R. P. (2005). Bayesian inference and attentional modulation in the visual cortex. Neuroreport 16, 1843–1848. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000183900.92901.fc

Rebuffi, S.-A., Bilen, H., and Vedaldi, A. (2017). “Learning multiple visual domains with residual adapters,” in Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems (Long Beach, CA), 506–516.

Ren, M., and Zemel, R. S. (2017). “End-to-end instance segmentation with recurrent attention,” in Proceedings of the IEEE Conference on Computer Vision and Pattern Recognition (Long Beach, CA), 6656–6664.

Reynolds, G. D., and Romano, A. C. (2016). The development of attention systems and working memory in infancy. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 10:15. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2016.00015

Reynolds, J. H., and Heeger, D. J. (2009). The normalization model of attention. Neuron 61, 168–185. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.01.002

Rizzolatti, G., Riggio, L., Dascola, I., and Umiltá, C. (1987). Reorienting attention across the horizontal and vertical meridians: evidence in favor of a premotor theory of attention. Neuropsychologia 25, 31–40. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(87)90041-8

Roelfsema, P. R., and Houtkamp, R. (2011). Incremental grouping of image elements in vision. Attent. Percept. Psychophys. 73, 2542–2572. doi: 10.3758/s13414-011-0200-0

Roelfsema, P. R., Lamme, V. A., and Spekreijse, H. (1998). Object-based attention in the primary visual cortex of the macaque monkey. Nature 395, 376–381. doi: 10.1038/26475

Rossi, A. F., and Paradiso, M. A. (1995). Feature-specific effects of selective visual attention. Vis. Res. 35, 621–634. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(94)00156-G

Russakovsky, O., Deng, J., Su, H., Krause, J., Satheesh, S., Ma, S., et al. (2015). ImageNet large scale visual recognition challenge. Int. J. Comput. Vis. 115, 211–252. doi: 10.1007/s11263-015-0816-y

Saalmann, Y. B., Pinsk, M. A., Wang, L., Li, X., and Kastner, S. (2012). The pulvinar regulates information transmission between cortical areas based on attention demands. Science 337, 753–756. doi: 10.1126/science.1223082

Saenz, M., Buracas, G. T., and Boynton, G. M. (2002). Global effects of feature-based attention in human visual cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 5, 631–632. doi: 10.1038/nn876

Sajedin, A., Menhaj, M. B., Vahabie, A.-H., Panzeri, S., and Esteky, H. (2019). Cholinergic modulation promotes attentional modulation in primary visual cortex-a modeling study. Sci. Rep. 9:20186. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-56608-3

Samuels, E. R., and Szabadi, E. (2008). Functional neuroanatomy of the noradrenergic locus coeruleus: its roles in the regulation of arousal and autonomic function part i: principles of functional organisation. Curr. Neuropharmacol. 6, 235–253. doi: 10.2174/157015908785777229

Schweisfurth, M. A., Schweizer, R., and Treue, S. (2014). Feature-based attentional modulation of orientation perception in somatosensation. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:519. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00519

Shapiro, K. L., Raymond, J., and Arnell, K. (1997). The attentional blink. Trends Cogn. Sci. 1, 291–296. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(97)01094-2

Sharma, S., Kiros, R., and Salakhutdinov, R. (2015). Action recognition using visual attention. arXiv [preprint] . arXiv:1511.04119.

Shenhav, A., Botvinick, M. M., and Cohen, J. D. (2013). The expected value of control: an integrative theory of anterior cingulate cortex function. Neuron 79, 217–240. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.07.007

Shipp, S. (2004). The brain circuitry of attention. Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, 223–230. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.03.004

Soto, D., Heinke, D., Humphreys, G. W., and Blanco, M. J. (2005). Early, involuntary top-down guidance of attention from working memory. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 31, 248–261. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.31.2.248

Soto, D., Hodsoll, J., Rotshtein, P., and Humphreys, G. W. (2008). Automatic guidance of attention from working memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 12, 342–348. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.05.007

Spence, C. (2009). Explaining the colavita visual dominance effect. Prog. Brain Res. 176, 245–258. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(09)17615-X

Spence, C., and Driver, J. (2004). Crossmodal Space and Crossmodal Attention . Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Sreenivasan, V., and Sridharan, D. (2019). Subcortical connectivity correlates selectively with attention's effects on spatial choice bias. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 116, 19711–19716. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1902704116

Stollenga, M. F., Masci, J., Gomez, F., and Schmidhuber, J. (2014). “Deep networks with internal selective attention through feedback connections,” in Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems (Montreal, QC), 3545–3553.

Strezoski, G., van Noord, N., and Worring, M. (2019). Many task learning with task routing. arXiv preprint arXiv:1903.12117 . doi: 10.1109/ICCV.2019.00146

Tatler, B. W., Baddeley, R. J., and Gilchrist, I. D. (2005). Visual correlates of fixation selection: effects of scale and time. Vis. Res. 45, 643–659. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2004.09.017

Toneva, M., and Wehbe, L. (2019). “Interpreting and improving natural-language processing (in machines) with natural language-processing (in the brain),” in Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems , 14928–14938.

Treisman, A., Vieira, A., and Hayes, A. (1992). Automaticity and preattentive processing. Am. J. Psychol. 105, 341–362. doi: 10.2307/1423032

Treue, S., and Trujillo, J. C. M. (1999). Feature-based attention influences motion processing gain in macaque visual cortex. Nature 399:575. doi: 10.1038/21176

Turk-Browne, N. B., Jungé, J. A., and Scholl, B. J. (2005). The automaticity of visual statistical learning. J. Exp. Psychol. 134, 552–564. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.134.4.552

Uncapher, M. R., and Rugg, M. D. (2005). Effects of divided attention on fmri correlates of memory encoding. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 17, 1923–1935. doi: 10.1162/089892905775008616

van Zoest, W., and Donk, M. (2005). The effects of salience on saccadic target selection. Vis. Cogn. 12, 353–375. doi: 10.1080/13506280444000229

Vaswani, A., Shazeer, N., Parmar, N., Uszkoreit, J., Jones, L., Gomez, A. N., et al. (2017). “Attention is all you need,” in Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems (Long Beach, CA), 5998–6008.

Wagner, A. D., Shannon, B. J., Kahn, I., and Buckner, R. L. (2005). Parietal lobe contributions to episodic memory retrieval. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 445–453. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.07.001

Wang, Q., Zhang, J., Song, S., and Zhang, Z. (2014). “Attentional neural network: Feature selection using cognitive feedback,” in Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems (Montreal, QC), 2033–2041.

Watanabe, W. (2003). Is subliminal learning really passive? Nature 422:36. doi: 10.1038/422036a

Wiegreffe, S., and Pinter, Y. (2019). Attention is not not explanation. arXiv [preprint] . arXiv:1908.04626.

Wolf, L., Guttmann, M., and Cohen-Or, D. (2007). “Non-homogeneous content-driven video-retargeting,” in 2007 IEEE 11th International Conference on Computer Vision (Rio de Janeiro), 1–6.

Wolfe, J. M., and Horowitz, T. S. (2004). What attributes guide the deployment of visual attention and how do they do it? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 5, 495–501. doi: 10.1038/nrn1411

Wood, S., Sage, J. R., Shuman, T., and Anagnostaras, S. G. (2014). Psychostimulants and cognition: a continuum of behavioral and cognitive activation. Pharmacol. Rev. 66, 193–221. doi: 10.1124/pr.112.007054

Wyatte, D., Jilk, D. J., and O'Reilly, R. C. (2014). Early recurrent feedback facilitates visual object recognition under challenging conditions. Front. Psychol. 5:674. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00674

Xu, H., and Saenko, K. (2016). “Ask, attend and answer: exploring question-guided spatial attention for visual question answering,” in European Conference on Computer Vision (Amsterdam: Springer), 451–466.

Xu, K., Ba, J., Kiros, R., Cho, K., Courville, A., Salakhudinov, R., et al. (2015). “Show, attend and tell: Neural image caption generation with visual attention,” in International Conference on Machine Learning (Lille), 2048–2057.

Yang, Z., He, X., Gao, J., Deng, L., and Smola, A. (2016). “Stacked attention networks for image question answering,” in Proceedings of the IEEE Conference on Computer Vision and Pattern Recognition (Las Vegas, NV), 21–29.

Zagoruyko, S., and Komodakis, N. (2016). Paying more attention to attention: improving the performance of convolutional neural networks via attention transfer. arXiv [preprint] . arXiv:1612.03928.

Zelinsky, G. J., Chen, Y., Ahn, S., Adeli, H., Yang, Z., Huang, L., et al. (2020). Predicting goal-directed attention control using inverse-reinforcement learning. arXiv [preprint] . arXiv:2001.11921.

Zhao, X., Li, H., Shen, X., Liang, X., and Wu, Y. (2018). “A modulation module for multi-task learning with applications in image retrieval,” in Proceedings of the European Conference on Computer Vision (ECCV) (Munich), 401–416.

Zhou, H., and Desimone, R. (2011). Feature-based attention in the frontal eye field and area V4 during visual search. Neuron 70, 1205–1217. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.04.032

Zhou, H., Schafer, R. J., and Desimone, R. (2016). Pulvinar-cortex interactions in vision and attention. Neuron 89, 209–220. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.11.034

Keywords: attention, artificial neural networks, machine learning, vision, memory, awareness

Citation: Lindsay GW (2020) Attention in Psychology, Neuroscience, and Machine Learning. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 14:29. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2020.00029

Received: 02 December 2019; Accepted: 23 April 2020; Published: 16 April 2020.

Reviewed by:

Copyright © 2020 Lindsay. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY) . The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Grace W. Lindsay, gracewlindsay@gmail.com

This article is part of the Research Topic

How Can Neuroscience Contribute to the Development of General Artificial Intelligence?

Questions? Call us: 

Email: 

  • How it works
  • Testimonials

Essay Writing

  • Essay service
  • Essay writers
  • College essay service
  • Write my essay
  • Pay for essay
  • Essay topics

Term Paper Writing

  • Term paper service
  • Buy term papers
  • Term paper help
  • Term paper writers
  • College term papers
  • Write my term paper
  • Pay for term paper
  • Term paper topic

Research Paper Writing

  • Research paper service
  • Buy research paper
  • Research paper help
  • Research paper writers
  • College research papers
  • Write my research paper
  • Pay for research paper
  • Research paper topics

Dissertation Writing

  • Dissertation service
  • Buy dissertation
  • Dissertation help
  • Dissertation writers
  • College thesis
  • Write my dissertation
  • Pay for dissertation
  • Dissertation topics

Other Services

  • Custom writing services
  • Speech writing service
  • Movie review writing
  • Editing service
  • Assignment writing
  • Article writing service
  • Book report writing
  • Book review writing

Popular request:

202 neuroscience topics to write about in depth.

August 25, 2021

Neuroscience as a medical discipline deals with the study of the human nervous system and how it impacts the general cognitive function, productivity, and behavior of the human brain. What neuroscience pays close attention to is how the various parts of the nervous system function and the various activities that go on within the brain with the reactions that are easily developed when something goes wrong within this part of the human body.

neuroscience topics

Neuroscience is another discipline of learning that requires the carrying out of extensive research in such a way that it allows for new learning, realizations and that will aid knowledge in the neuro field. There are so many aspects that could be touched across while writing a research paper on neuroscience. As a field of study, neuroscience on its own is covered with interesting neuroscience topics. Some of the topics in neuroscience look into behavioral neuroscience topics, neuroscience research paper topics, and some other cool neuroscience topics that will be subsequently highlighted.

Neuroscience Topics for Research Paper

As a student who is preparing to undertake your research paper writing in the neuro field, it’s important to note that there are myriads of topics that could serve as good research topics within this field. All that is required is that you have a basic understanding of the particular topic or area you end up specializing in as it will enable you to turn out a well-researched and properly done paper. Some of the topics to look into are.

  • A case study of the developmental stage of Alzheimer’s in the brain
  • Is Alzheimer’s hereditary? A case study of hereditary diseases
  • The distinction between ADHD and Dyslexia and how individual patients react to them
  • Understanding how the brain assimilates and records sound waves
  • The neurological effect of excessive brain stress
  • Health causes of brain tumor
  • The importance of mindfulness and how it enhances the brain function
  • How the brain enables the enhancement of language skills
  • Study of the root causes of Alzheimer’s
  • Effective ways to manage Alzheimer’s at an early stage
  • Understanding the relationship between the brain and stress
  • The role of the gene in influencing the growth of Alzheimer’s in one
  • A case study of some of the mental abilities that open up while aging
  • A study of how sounds and words are received in the brain
  • Does Alzheimer’s disease start at a young age?
  • The health challenges that often result in wrongly done brain surgery
  • The importance of Head CT before brain surgery
  • The role Head CT places in the medical field
  • The importance of artificial intelligence in brain surgery
  • The importance of healthy eating to the brain
  • Impulsivity: understanding how it is formulated in the brain
  • Amnesia and how memory loss operates
  • A look into some of the medical ways to manage Amnesia
  • How memory can be reintroduced to an Amnesiac patient
  • Understanding emotions, attention, and memory and how they’re formulated by the brain
  • A case study of the structural differences in the brain
  • The root causes of Dementia in people

Interesting Topics in Neuroscience

For most students, the challenge usually lies in how to formulate an interesting neuroscience topic that not only will enable them to score better points in their grade but will be interesting enough to keep a reader engaged with the paper. But, finding these topics or knowing how to formulate them is an essential aspect of preparing a research paper. Here, is a compilation of some of the interesting topics in the field of neuroscience.

  • Brainstorming and its health benefits to the human brain
  • Understanding the role of the brain and its reception to emotions
  • A case study of how different music tones are received in the brain
  • A look into how PTSD is developed in the human brain
  • The role of chronic fatigue and how it impacts the human brain
  • Ways the brain struggles during traumatic experiences
  • Does music affect the intellectual capacity of a student?
  • A study of the brain capacity to function between children and teenagers
  • The various causes of head injury and how they develop in the brain
  • What is nerve stimulation and how is its function in the brain carried out?
  • What are the root cause of autism in young children and ways they can be handled
  • The importance of memory exercise for patients with a minor head injury
  • Some medically advised ways to handle head injuries
  • The difference and similarities of brain disorder and brain damage
  • The causes of bipolar in adults
  • Some of the ways through which Bipolar can be managed
  • A distinction and similarities between Bipolar I and Bipolar II
  • Mental health: ways to manage mental health better for sound brain function
  • How the brain processes hurt
  • How the human brain processes the reception of cool images
  • The importance of scenic views towards enhancing the brain
  • The medical reason behind the implantation of electricals into the human brain
  • A study of other body parts injuries that could affect the brain
  • A look into how depression is formed in the brain
  • The distinction between autism and dyslexia

Behavioral Neuroscience Research Topics

Behavioral neuroscience deals primarily with the study of how biology impacts the psychological reactions in the human brain. In many cases, it is either referred to also as biopsychology or biological psychology, or psychobiology. It focuses on the study of human traits and how they manifest in human behavioral patterns. There are so many topics that fall within this category of neuroscience. Some of the topics are.

  • A study of how Schizophrenia is developed in the human brain
  • A study of how the human brain constructs the mind
  • The benefits of brain rehabilitation programs
  • The experiences of children with autism and how behavior impacts their health
  • The role of the brain in the Motivation to eat
  • How stress reduces the brain’s ability to function
  • How stress can hamper sexual motivation in women
  • A case study of some of the brain reactions that encourage social behaviors
  • Dopamine and the role it plays within the human brain
  • The importance of audiovisual to the brain
  • The different roles audio and visual qualities play in enhancing the brain
  • The emotional study of the human brain
  • How does the brain process type of speech
  • How the brain processes the ability to recognize a person
  • Behavioral patterns that could reduce the risk of depression
  • A study of how drug abuse can affect the smooth functioning of dopamine
  • A look into how to better manage activities that require the head contact
  • Ways to promote good brain functioning while engaging in activities
  • Mental health challenges and ways to manage them
  • The behavioral pattern of people living with ADHD
  • Understanding the relationship between ADHD patients and their behaviors
  • Ways behavior influences ADHD
  • How much Dyslexia is impacted by behavior?
  • Best ways to manage Dyslexic patients
  • Effective ways to promote good mental health in adults and children

Neuroscience Research Paper Topics

There are still so many topics that could be formulated from neuroscience. As a field that deals primarily with psychology and the brain, it, therefore, means that so many aspects of it can be looked into while carrying out research writing. If you are studying neuroscience and in need of topics to support your research, below are some other neuroscience research paper topics to look into while preparing your research writing.

  • How to point out the developmental stage of Alzheimer’s in old people
  • Stages of Schizophrenia in people
  • What are some of the medical and behavioral causes of schizophrenia
  • A study on how Alzheimer’s patients behave
  • How to identify High IQ in people
  • A study of some of the prevalent psychological challenges faced by people with high IQ
  • How worrying affects the function of the brain
  • Gut bacteria and neurological problems
  • Understanding learning and memory
  • What is neuroplasticity?
  • Does Parkinson’s have any effect on the brain?
  • Addiction and how it affects the brain?
  • Medical treatments for managing brain disorder
  • A study of medical treatments for ADHD
  • The correlation between aging and memory loss
  • Is depression a mental issue or brain issue?
  • Is Alzheimer’s hereditary or is it caused by a virus?
  • Symptoms of the developmental stage of chronic fatigue
  • Is Cannabis damaging to the brain?
  • A study of Cognitive offloading in the brain
  • A study of the developmental stage of autism
  • How to manage autistic patients
  • The importance of hibernation to the brain
  • The difference between hibernation and hallucination
  • Impact of brain disorder on language

Cognitive Neuroscience Research Topics

Cognitive neuroscience is a branch of neuroscience that studies the various biological processes that inform brain cognition and reception. If you are preparing to write your academic essay in this subfield of neuroscience, the first thing to always note is the topic you’ll be working with. Here are some cognitive neuroscience topics.

  • How the human brain is wired
  • How speech is formed in children
  • What are the ways through which excessive internet consumption affects the human brain function
  • The link between internet abuse and brain loss
  • A study of chronic pain and how it affects the brain
  • Does the abuse of drugs affect the brain?
  • The use of marijuana by teenagers and its brain effects
  • The effect of addiction on the brain
  • Understanding how the human brain internalizes emotions
  • Does hallucination cause any brain deficit?
  • A study of cognitive disorder in adults
  • Does cognitive disorder occur in young people?
  • Effects of opium on the brain
  • Understanding autism spectrum disorder in children
  • How to identify autism in children at an early stage
  • Medical procedures for managing autism at an early stage
  • Social limitation of autistic children
  • Why do People with ADHD have trouble paying attention
  • Impulsivity as a major challenge for ADHD patients
  • Effects of the medication in treating ADHD
  • The treatment plan for ADHD
  • ADHD and how it affects cognitive behavior in adults
  • A study of ADHD as a cognitive disorder
  • Autism is a cognitive disorder in children
  • How Autistic and Dyslexic students manage learning disabilities

Controversial Topics in Neuroscience

Every aspect of the neuroscience field can be considered controversial due to the very sensitive issues that relate to the programming and functioning of the human psychology and brain that it deals predominantly on. In case you are preparing your research paper on neuroscience and would prefer to look into some of the controversial areas in the field, here is a list of some of them.

  • How deep learning affects the brain
  • A study of how too much straining of the brain causes mental disorder
  • Lack of exercise as the root cause of Dementia
  • Hereditary brain disorders
  • How to care for hereditary neural issues
  • How poor nutrition exacerbates the brain
  • Causes of cognitive decline in adults
  • How visually impaired people perceive physical items
  • How sufficient sleep impacts the brain
  • Root causes of depression in people and how it affects the brain
  • What are neurogenerative diseases
  • How can neurogenerative diseases be handled
  • Ways to regulate the growth of Alzheimer’s
  • Clinical trials for the cure of Alzheimer’s disease
  • The role of CT scans and MRI machines in treating brain injuries
  • Medically advised ways to outgrow ADHD
  • How to boost the brain through regular brain exercise
  • A study of the benefits of consistent study
  • Can Alzheimer’s be treated with other drugs?
  • Sleep deprivation and its effect on the brain
  • Effects of isolation on the human brain
  • How information is processed in the brain
  • Down Syndrome and how it affects cognitive reception
  • Dysfunctional psychological issues due to cerebral palsy
  • A study of the impacts of antidepressant

Social Neuroscience Topics

Social neuroscience as a branch of neuroscience has some resemblance to behavioral neuroscience. The distinction is that social neuroscience deals primarily with how the brain performs social processes. In this area of the study, so many parts can be touched across because it involves a wide range of topics that can be examined within this field. Social neuroscience deals with social interactions, and every other form of social activities and behavior that impacts the brain. Here are some of the topics to look into when writing your research on social neuroscience.

  • Narcissism as a flawed personality trait
  • How the nervous system reacts to hormonal imbalance
  • How does chronic fatigue affect the ability to socialize
  • How do emotions affect our attitude to things?
  • How well can visually impaired people learn to drive?
  • Challenges of physically impaired people in performing activities
  • How does the brain react due to physical trauma?
  • Psychological trauma effects in victims?
  • How does the brain process music?
  • How can stress levels cause memory loss?
  • How does the brain process conversations?
  • What are the ways PTSD affects an individual
  • The effects of trauma on accident victims
  • How to handle trauma at an early stage
  • Medically trusted treatment plans for PTSD
  • The distinction between PTSD and depression
  • How to identify depression in young adults
  • Challenges in the brain when the dopamine isn’t functional
  • How the brain realigns after trauma
  • Where does the brain store memories?
  • Can the brain store memories for a long period?
  • Effective ways of treating temporary memory loss
  • Effective care package for patients living with temporary memory loss
  • How medication can damage aspects of the brain
  • Some of the brain rehabilitation process
  • A study of permanent, temporary memory and their causes.

Hot Topics in Neuroscience

While some topics are considered controversial, there are other topics still within this particular discipline that are considered hot topics because of the level of engagements they continue to bring. Some of the topics that fall well within this category include.

  • Do professional athletes suffer brain injuries?
  • The treatment plan for professional athletes with brain injuries
  • What are the best treatment plans for Schizophrenia
  • How the left hemisphere of the brain functions
  • Can schizophrenia occur in children?
  • What are the sufficient treatment plans for PTSD
  • Psychological trauma and how to handle them in children
  • Ways to prevent depression in young children
  • A study of the developmental stage of brain
  • How does the brain articulate speech?
  • How the brain programs knowledge for visually impaired people
  • The risks of developing severe brain injury
  • A study of postpartum depression in nursing mothers
  • The medical effects that could arise from antidepressants
  • Drug abuse and the lack of self-control
  • A study of healthy eating habits that promotes the brain
  • A study of unhealthy foods that affects the brain
  • The impact of loss on the human psyche
  • Language formation process in adults
  • The challenges adults face while learning new languages
  • The importance of language learning in childhood
  • How the brain processes and receives different types of speech
  • Important ways on how to care for the brain
  • The brain is the most important aspect in the human body.

If you are looking for assistance on how to prepare your college essay writing and you’ll require the professional assistance of expert writers online to help you produce quality essays at cheap fee and fast turnaround time, look no further for there are experts in this field that will develop interesting and easy to assimilate research essays for you.

responsibility essay

Take a break from writing.

Top academic experts are here for you.

  • How To Write An Autobiography Guideline And Useful Advice
  • 182 Best Classification Essay Topics To Learn And Write About
  • How To Manage Stress In College: Top Practical Tips  
  • How To Write A Narrative Essay: Definition, Tips, And A Step-by-Step Guide
  • How To Write Article Review Like Professional
  • Great Problem Solution Essay Topics
  • Creating Best Stanford Roommate Essay
  • Costco Essay – Best Writing Guide
  • How To Quote A Dialogue
  • Wonderful Expository Essay Topics
  • Research Paper Topics For 2020
  • Interesting Persuasive Essay Topics
  • How It Works
  • PhD thesis writing
  • Master thesis writing
  • Bachelor thesis writing
  • Dissertation writing service
  • Dissertation abstract writing
  • Thesis proposal writing
  • Thesis editing service
  • Thesis proofreading service
  • Thesis formatting service
  • Coursework writing service
  • Research paper writing service
  • Architecture thesis writing
  • Computer science thesis writing
  • Engineering thesis writing
  • History thesis writing
  • MBA thesis writing
  • Nursing dissertation writing
  • Psychology dissertation writing
  • Sociology thesis writing
  • Statistics dissertation writing
  • Buy dissertation online
  • Write my dissertation
  • Cheap thesis
  • Cheap dissertation
  • Custom dissertation
  • Dissertation help
  • Pay for thesis
  • Pay for dissertation
  • Senior thesis
  • Write my thesis

121 Original Neuroscience Research Topics

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Now, wouldn’t it be great if you had a list of awesome neuroscience research topics to choose from? Our PhD dissertation help would definitely make writing a thesis or dissertation a lot easier. Well, the good news is that we have a long list of neuroscience paper topics for you right here.

The list of topics is updated periodically, so you will surely be able to find a unique topic; something that nobody has though of yet. And yes, you can use any of our topics for free.

Writing a Neuroscience Dissertation

To write a good dissertation, you need more than just our interesting neuroscience topics. Your supervisor expects you to make some progress pretty quickly, so you really need all the help you can get. You can get all the assistance you need to get started quickly from our dissertation experts and you’ll also find the following guide useful:

Set up your project and conduct the necessary research and data analysis. Don’t forget to think about an interesting, captivating thesis statement. Start by writing the first chapter of the dissertation, the introduction. This will provide your readers with comprehensive background information about your study. Write the Literature Review chapter. This will take some time, especially if you are dealing with a popular subject. Write the Methodology chapter. This is basically an iteration and in-depth description of each and every method you have used to collect the data. Write the Results chapter. In this chapter, you will present your readers the results of your research. You don’t need to provide your own take on the data yet. Next comes the Discussion (or Analysis) chapter. This is where you are free to discuss your results and show your readers how they support your thesis. Finally, the Conclusion chapter wraps everything up. You can summarize your methods, results and analysis and make it clear that your paper has answered all the relevant research questions. Write the References section and the Appendices section. Edit and proofread your work thoroughly to make sure you don’t lose points over some minor mistakes – or have our expert proofreaders and editors do it for you.

This step-by-step guide applies to any thesis or dissertation. However, before you even get this far, you need a great topic to start with. Fortunately, we have 121 brand new topics for you right here on this page.

Interesting Neuroscience Topics

If you are looking for some of the most interesting neuroscience topics, you have definitely arrived at the right place. Our experts have put together the best list of ideas for you:

  • Research the occurrence of cerebrovascular disease in the United States
  • What causes a headache?
  • An in-depth look at muscular dystrophy
  • The causes of multiple sclerosis
  • Talk about neuroregeneration
  • Define cognitive neuroscience
  • Everything about dementia
  • Study brain development from birth to age 2
  • What causes Parkinson’s disease?
  • The function of peripheral nerves
  • What are vestibular disorders?
  • Pain and the science behind it
  • An in-depth analysis of stem cells

Engaging Topics in Neuroscience

Are you looking for some engaging topics in neuroscience? If you want the best ideas, all you have to do is take a look at the following list and take your pick:

  • Research the Down syndrome
  • A closer look at ADHD
  • What causes brain tumors?
  • What causes epilepsy episodes?
  • Research the occurrence of schizophrenia in the UK
  • An in-depth look at brain stimulation
  • Treating severe depression in young adults
  • Improving memory in the adult population
  • The importance of sleep for brain health
  • Mapping the human brain

Comprehensive Neuroscience Topic for Every Student

The nice thing about our blog is that we have a comprehensive neuroscience topic for every student. Even better, all our topics are relatively simple, so you don’t have to spend a lot of time doing research:

  • The future of brain implants
  • The processes behind depression
  • The role of dopamine
  • How are emotions created?
  • Love starts in your brain, not your heart
  • ADHD behavior and brain activity
  • Effects of illegal drugs on dopamine production
  • How does dyslexia manifest itself?
  • Early stages of Schizophrenia
  • The link between gut bacteria and the brain
  • Studying the brains of people with a high IQ

Neuroscience Research Questions

The best way to get ideas for your next paper is to take a look at some original neuroscience research questions. Here are some that should get you started right away:

  • How do brain tumors cause damage?
  • What causes substance addiction?
  • What role does the brain play in autistic spectrum disorders?
  • Does being a vegetarian influence your brain?
  • What causes chronic migraines?
  • Why is Pierre Paul Broca’s work important?
  • Why is stress so dangerous for the brain?
  • How do genes influence the onset of Alzheimer’s disease?
  • What can cause a brain tumor?
  • Does music affect the human brain?
  • Can repeated head injuries damage the brain? (think about modern sports)
  • What does being Bipolar I mean?

Easy Neuroscience Paper Topics

Our experts have created a list of easy neuroscience paper topics for you. You could start writing your thesis in no time if you choose one of these great ideas:

  • What causes epilepsy?
  • A closer look at Alzheimer’s disease
  • What can cause a loss of feeling?
  • The effects of dementia on the brain
  • The symptoms of Parkinson’s disease
  • What can cause memory loss?
  • Mitigating headaches without medication
  • The effects of a mild stroke
  • Talk about Amyotrophic Lateral Sclerosis
  • What can cause a lack of coordination?

Neuroscience Research Topics for College Students

We have a list of awesome neuroscience research topics for college students and you can use any one of them for free. Take a look at our best ideas yet:

  • Can the brain be linked to substance abuse?
  • How does the brain recognize people?
  • Latest development in brain surgery
  • An in-depth look at neuroplasticity
  • Innovative medication for treating brain disorders
  • Treating Alzheimer’s in 2023
  • How damaging is Cannabis for the brain?

Cognitive Neuroscience Research Topics

If you want to talk about something in cognitive neuroscience, we have put together the best and most interesting cognitive neuroscience research topics:

  • The role played by neurons in our body
  • What is Magnetoencephalography?
  • How difficult is it to map the entire brain?
  • Define consciousness from a neurological POV
  • How does our brain affect our perception?
  • Discuss Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation procedures
  • Latest advancements in Functional magnetic resonance imaging

Brain Research Topics

Brain research is a very interesting thing to talk about, especially since we are still struggling to understand how certain things work. Take a look at some amazing brain research topics:

  • Study the brain development of an infant
  • Brain tumor stages
  • The effect of social media on the human brain
  • Multiple sclerosis treatment options
  • What can cause muscular dystrophy?
  • Discuss 3 cerebrovascular diseases
  • Interesting breakthroughs in cellular neuroscience
  • Talk about our brain’s problem-solving abilities
  • The effects of sugar on brain chemistry

Neurobiology Topics

We agree, researching a topic in neurobiology is not easy. However, with the right neurobiology topics, you could write an awesome thesis without spending years working on it:

  • Research the role of the amygdala
  • What are brain neurotransmitters?
  • The causes of posttraumatic stress disorder
  • How do we recognize a bipolar disorder?
  • The importance of hormones
  • Talk about experimental psychology

Behavioral Neuroscience Research Topics

Do you want to write your dissertation on a behavioral neuroscience topic? Our experts have compiled a list of the most interesting behavioral neuroscience research topics for you:

  • The processes behind sensation
  • How does the brain control our movement?
  • An in-depth look at motivated behavior
  • Best way to diagnose a sleep disorder
  • Improving success at academic activities
  • How does your brain perceive the environment?

Cool Neuroscience Topics

We have some very cool neuroscience topics right here and the good news is that they’re all relatively easy. The list has been updated recently and new topics have been added:

  • Effects of plant-based diets
  • The life and work of Cornelia Bargmann
  • Discuss a breakthrough in neurotech
  • 3D brain function mapping
  • Discuss the importance of brain implants
  • The life and work of Róbert Bárány

Controversial Topics in Neuroscience

Just like any other field, neuroscience has its controversies. And what better way to start a dissertation than finding the most controversial topics in neuroscience:

  • Discuss the Bayesian brain theory
  • Ethics behind wearable brain gadgets
  • Discuss postnatal neurogenesis
  • Can our brain “deep learn”?
  • Invasive brain imaging procedures
  • How do we differentiate between good and bad?

Hot Topics in Neuroscience

Did you know that getting hot topics in neuroscience is not overly difficult? This section of our list of topics is updated periodically, so you can definitely find an original idea right here:

  • Electrical brain stimulation methods
  • Define the concept of Free Will
  • Talk about hereditary brain disorders
  • How is speech formed?
  • Can our brain hibernate?
  • What causes aggressive behavior?

Current Topics in Neuroscience

The best way to make your thesis interesting is to write about something that is of great interest. This means you need to choose one of our current topics in neuroscience:

  • Cerebellar Neurons that can help you lose weight
  • Effects of a meat-based diet
  • Latest brain mapping technology
  • CT scans in 2023
  • Brain implants that can control a computer
  • An in-depth look at super-agers

Complex Neurological Research Topics

Are you looking for some complex neurological research topics? If you want to give a difficult topic a try, don’t hesitate to choose one of these excellent ideas:

  • An in-depth look at the Demyelinating disease
  • The effects of a cerebrovascular stroke
  • Bioterrorism in 2023
  • Legal issues in neurology
  • Dopamine’s link to aggressiveness
  • Brain changes that lead to alcohol addiction

Can You Help Me With My Thesis?

So, can you help me with my thesis? Of course, we can help you with much more than some interesting neuroscience research paper topics. Our experienced professionals are ready to give you the best dissertation assistance on the Internet and make sure you get a top score on your paper. All our university educated ENL writers have extensive experience writing dissertations on any subject and topic you can imagine. These cheap dissertation writing services can deliver a final paper in no time, so don’t hesitate to get in touch with us even if you are on a tight deadline.

Our PhD-holding writers and editors are ready to spring into action right now. We can help you with the research, as well as with thesis writing, editing and proofreading. Moreover, we can write a high quality research paper for any high school, college or university student. Your professor will love our work – guaranteed. Our company has 24/7 customer support, so you can order custom academic content online at any time of day or night. What are you waiting for? Give us a try and get a discount!

epidemiology topics

Leave a Reply Cancel reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

Comment * Error message

Name * Error message

Email * Error message

Save my name, email, and website in this browser for the next time I comment.

As Putin continues killing civilians, bombing kindergartens, and threatening WWIII, Ukraine fights for the world's peaceful future.

Ukraine Live Updates

NeuroTracker logo

9 Major Neuroscience Breakthroughs of 2020

December 21, 2020

Featured Post

Although it’s been a difficult year for most fields of science, the golden age of neuroscience has continued to thrive at an accelerating pace throughout 2020. In particular there have been multiple sci-fi like breakthroughs for mapping our brains, major developments for improving human health into older age, and the dawn of a new era of AI-based neuroscience. Lets take a look at 9 of the top neuroscience discoveries of this past year.

1. Live 3D Brain Function Mapping

Earlier this year MIT scientists developed a new technique to pair structural mapping (brain anatomy) with functional mapping (how the brain behaves) - the first time this has been properly achieved. In addition, this has been done in live mice, with the mapping performed across mouse brain regions in real-time. This video gives an idea of how fascinating it is to see the coupling of brain structures and live activity changing in response to a mouse being shown different images.

The vanguard technique combines third-harmonic generation ( THG ) three-photon microscopy with retinotopic mapping , allowing activity to be observed through deep brain tissue via electrical signatures.

It also delivers stunning resolution, allowing individual neurons and their substructures to be studied, as well as fine blood vessels and myelin – a kind of insulator known to be a critical factor in brain processing speed.  

This study focused on the visual centers of the brain, but the same method can be used to study other regions. It promises to be a powerful tool for understanding differences in healthy and diseased brain states, as well as how the brain responds to environmental stimulation.

2. Seeing Decisions Being Made in the Brain

Stanford University made a key breakthrough with a new bifocal microscopy technique called COSMOS . Their work captured movies of neural activity across the whole of the cerebral cortex of a mouse brain.

These signals were recorded by essentially filming the brain from three different angles, then computationally extracting signals to provide a live video of macroscopic activity over the left and right hemispheres. Here is a sample where you literally see the remarkable electrical storm of a real brain in action.

As the cortex handles complex higher-level cognitive functions, more mysterious behaviors like decision-making processes can now start to be unraveled in a global way. For example for understanding of the relationship of decisions dependent on sensory perception and motor function (think about what’s involved in deciding which way to dodge an oncoming car).

The researchers also expect COSMOS to be a low-cost method for screening the effects of psychiatric drugs, so that they can be developed to be more functionally effective.

3. Sleep Breakthrough for Artificial Brains

As we’ve covered in a previous blog , a major breakthrough for Google’s Deep Mind artificial intelligence program came through mimicking the neo-cortical columns of the human mind. This led to vastly increased intelligence using a fraction of the computing power. As a result this human-modelled AI has now surpassed the world’s best chess, Go and then eSports players at their own games.

Though not fully understood, sleep provides a critical function for mammalian and human brains, with serious problems occurring whenever sleep deprivation is endured. This year Los Alamos National Laboratory discovered that the spiking computational networks of AI systems also suffer a kind of sleep deprivation, becoming unstable when performing for long periods without rests. Yet, when put into a network state similar to the brainwaves we experience during sleep, optimal performance was restored.

This may not sound like such a big deal, but advancements in AI are likely to transform the way we all our lives. The findings also hint that the merging disciplines of neuroscience and AI field could yield a new era of super smart computers.

4. Tiny Implant Allows Paralyzed Patients to Control a Computer

A minuscule brain device has been used to improve quality of life patients with severe upper limb paralysis caused by motor neuron disease. Carried out at the University of Melbourne, this trial implanted the new micro technology inside the brains of the participants.

The device called Stentrode™ was inserted through keyhole surgery into the neck, and from there moved into the motor cortex via blood vessels. This minimally invasive method avoids the associated risks and recovery complications of open brain surgery.

The implant uses wireless technology to relay specific neuronal activity into a computer, where it is converted in actions based off the intentions of the patients. Amazingly, this tiny chip allowed the patients to perform actions like click and zoom, and write with 93% accuracy, helping them do things we take for granted like text, email and shop online.  

It's very early days still, but the minimally invasive nature of the treatment shows the great potential for micro neurotechnologies to help aid people with all kinds of cognitive impairments.

5. Neuroscientists Turn Normal Neurons Into Regenerating Ones

In 2018 we reported that scientists learned how to reprogram stem cells into specific neurons. This year researchers from four different US universities have taken a bigger step towards the holy grail of life extension. By identifying genes networks that regulate cellular regeneration, they have been able to manipulate normal cells to turn into progenitor cells , which can morph into any cell type to replace dying cells.

Their proof of concept was carried out with the glial cells of Zebra fish, effectively converting them into stem cells which then detected and restored damaged retinal cells to recover impaired vision.

Cell death, or apoptosis , is a plays a big role in the inevitably of natural aging in humans. The researchers believe that the process for regenerating neurons in the brain will be similar. If successful it will have vast implications for conditions such as Alzheimer’s Disease, where large regions of the brain can be lost to the death of neurons. It may also play a role in preventing the many side-effects of natural aging in the brain, for longer and healthier living in peak shape into old age.

6. Preventing Neurodegeneration

Rather than replaced dying cells, scientists at Heidelberg University have identified key processes involved in the death of brain cells, called neurodegeneration . It involved uncovering the process by which cellular glutamate uptake prevents cell death in healthy people, yet becomes inactive in diseased state like stroke, where oxygen supply to brain cells becomes restricted.

In effect this leads to cell killing themselves off simply because they are not getting the correct chemical signals to tell them to stay alive. The researchers then developed a special class of inhibitors that can step in and deactivate the cellular ‘death complex’ before it occurs.

The inhibitors showed to be highly effective at protecting nerve cells, hopefully leading to a new class of treatments options for neurodegenerative diseases.

7. Parkinson’s Disease is One of Two Different Diseases

Aarhus University researchers have used advanced PET and MRI imaging techniques to reveal Parkinson’s disease to actually be either of two different variants of the disease .

In one variant the disease starts in the intestines, going on to spread to the brain through neural connections. In the other, it starts in the brain and then moves into the intestines and other organs. This video gives a great overview.

Though not curative, it’s a major step in the right direction for being able to identify early stage onset for preventative measures. For example, it may lead to treatments which prevent the disease from even making it into the brain altogether, where the effects then become debilitating over time. It is also another key piece in the puzzle of the powerful symbioses between our intestines and our mind, known scientifically as the gut-brain axis .

8. AI Advances the Challenging Diagnoses of Brain Injuries

Scientists at the University of Cambridge and Imperial College London have developed a new type of AI algorithm that can detect, differentiate and identify different types of brain injuries from topographical CT scan data.

CT scans collect a huge amount of data which can take experts hours to analyze, and this needs to include the collective evaluation of multiple scans over time in order to track recovery trajectories or disease progression. This new AI tool appears to better than human experts at detecting such changes, as well as being far quicker and cheaper.

For example, their research showed the software to be highly effective at automatically quantifying the progression of multiple types of brain lesions, helping predict which lesions would get larger. The innovative application of this type of AI to assist human analysis is likely to be first of many that will transform medical diagnostics in cost-effective ways.

9. Secret of Super-Agers Discovered

Super-agers are individuals whose cognitive skills are way past their peers in old age, retaining youthful mental abilities well into their 70s and 80s. Until now the secret to retaining their peak shape has been little understood.

University Hospital Cologne and the Research Center Juelich have discovered a key difference in their biology . Using PET scans they revealed that super-agers have markedly increased resistance to tau and amyloid proteins . Until recent years these proteins have proven difficult to study.

Super-agers also have lower levels of tau and amyloid pathology, which in turns leads to various kinds of neurodegeneration in most people in their later years. It’s now been identified that reduced resistance to tau and amyloid accumulation is a primary biological factor for the loss of peak cognitive shape.

New research can be focused on these processes to find ways to possibly cure mental decline generally, as well as help develop therapeutics to protect against forms of dementia that are already occurring.

We hope you found these neuroscience highlight interesting. If you're keen to learn more about the remarkable pace of progress in neuroscience, then also read our blogs on the highlights of the previous three years.

Top 3 Mind-blowing Neuroscience Stories of 2019

5 Neuroscience Breakthroughs of 2018

7 Major Developments in Neuroscience of 2017

NeuroTracker logo

latests news from us

Related Posts

Check out rare research on the far transfer effects of cognitive training to pro sports performance.

Prepare to be surprised just how much water influences your grey matter.

Discover neuroscience findings on the influence of sports on high-level cognition.

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

NeuroTrackerX NeuroTrackerX is a cognitive training program designed to assess and improve mental performance.

Instagram link

Useful Links

*Elite athletes and skilled specialists from teams and organizations like these. All trademarks and logos are intellectual property and owned by the respective organizations listed, not NeuroTracker.*

** NeuroTracker is used in various peer reviewed medical research and applications under the guidance of a licensed medical professional. NeuroTracker is not intended to be substituted for professional medical advice, diagnosis, or treatment.

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • View all journals
  • Explore content
  • About the journal
  • Publish with us
  • Sign up for alerts

Collection  12 March 2023

Top 100 in Neuroscience - 2022

This collection highlights our most downloaded* neuroscience papers published in 2022. Featuring authors from around the world, these papers showcase valuable research from an international community.

You can also view the top papers across various subject areas here .

*Data obtained from SN Insights, which is based on Digital Science's Dimensions.

image of blue neurons

Chlamydia pneumoniae can infect the central nervous system via the olfactory and trigeminal nerves and contributes to Alzheimer’s disease risk

  • Jenny A. K. Ekberg

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Curcumin and metformin synergistically modulate peripheral and central immune mechanisms of pain

  • Peththa Wadu Dasuni Wasana
  • Pasarapa Towiwat

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Efficient dendritic learning as an alternative to synaptic plasticity hypothesis

  • Shiri Hodassman

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

NU-9 improves health of hSOD1 G93A mouse upper motor neurons in vitro, especially in combination with riluzole or edaravone

  • Mukesh Gautam
  • P. Hande Ozdinler

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Cellular analysis of SOD1 protein-aggregation propensity and toxicity: a case of ALS with slow progression harboring homozygous SOD1-D92G mutation

  • Masanori Sawamura
  • Keiko Imamura
  • Ryosuke Takahashi

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Human iPSC co-culture model to investigate the interaction between microglia and motor neurons

  • Björn F. Vahsen
  • Elizabeth Gray
  • Kevin Talbot

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Reduced variability of bursting activity during working memory

  • Mikael Lundqvist
  • Earl K. Miller

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Psilocybin reduces low frequency oscillatory power and neuronal phase-locking in the anterior cingulate cortex of awake rodents

  • Caroline T. Golden
  • Paul Chadderton

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Transcellular propagation of fibrillar α-synuclein from enteroendocrine to neuronal cells requires cell-to-cell contact and is Rab35-dependent

  • Paulla Vieira Rodrigues
  • João Vitor Pereira de Godoy
  • Matheus de Castro Fonseca

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

High-fat diet causes mechanical allodynia in the absence of injury or diabetic pathology

  • Jessica A. Tierney
  • Calvin D. Uong
  • Michael D. Burton

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Recovery of anosmia in hamsters infected with SARS-CoV-2 is correlated with repair of the olfactory epithelium

  • Rachel A. Reyna
  • Megumi Kishimoto-Urata
  • Junki Maruyama

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Optimizing intact skull intrinsic signal imaging for subsequent targeted electrophysiology across mouse visual cortex

  • Armel Nsiangani
  • Joseph Del Rosario
  • Bilal Haider

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Symptoms of depression change with olfactory function

  • Agnieszka Sabiniewicz
  • Leonie Hoffmann
  • Thomas Hummel

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Heterogeneous fates of simultaneously-born neurons in the cortical ventricular zone

  • Elia Magrinelli
  • Natalia Baumann
  • Esther Klingler

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Movement observation activates motor cortex in fibromyalgia patients: a fNIRS study

  • Eleonora Gentile
  • Antonio Brunetti
  • Marina de Tommaso

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Transcriptomic analysis of human sensory neurons in painful diabetic neuropathy reveals inflammation and neuronal loss

  • Bradford E. Hall
  • Emma Macdonald
  • Ashok B. Kulkarni

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Cerebral cortical processing time is elongated in human brain evolution

  • Kosuke Itoh
  • Naho Konoike
  • Katsuki Nakamura

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Establishment and characterization of human pluripotent stem cells-derived brain organoids to model cerebellar diseases

  • Daniel Henriques
  • Liliana S. Mendonça

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Changes in the excitability of primary hippocampal neurons following exposure to 3.0 GHz radiofrequency electromagnetic fields

  • Ibtissam Echchgadda
  • Jody C. Cantu
  • Bennett L. Ibey

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Disrupted connectivity in the olfactory bulb-entorhinal cortex-dorsal hippocampus circuit is associated with recognition memory deficit in Alzheimer’s disease model

  • Morteza Salimi
  • Farhad Tabasi
  • Mohammad Reza Raoufy

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Enhanced firing of locus coeruleus neurons and SK channel dysfunction are conserved in distinct models of prodromal Parkinson’s disease

  • Lina A. Matschke
  • Marlene A. Komadowski
  • Niels Decher

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Parvalbumin expressing interneurons control spike-phase coupling of hippocampal cells to theta oscillations

  • Michael Strüber
  • Jonas-Frederic Sauer
  • Marlene Bartos

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

A psychophysical and neuroimaging analysis of genital hedonic sensation in men

  • Gerben B. Ruesink
  • Francis P. McGlone
  • Janniko R. Georgiadis

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Bupivacaine reduces GlyT1 expression by potentiating the p-AMPKα/BDNF signalling pathway in spinal astrocytes of rats

  • Hanxiang Ma

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Prolonged and extended impacts of SARS-CoV-2 on the olfactory neurocircuit

  • Shinji Urata
  • Slobodan Paessler

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Voltage imaging in the olfactory bulb using transgenic mouse lines expressing the genetically encoded voltage indicator ArcLight

  • Jelena Platisa
  • Hongkui Zeng
  • Douglas A. Storace

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

GABAergic CA1 neurons are more stable following context changes than glutamatergic cells

  • Peter J. Schuette
  • Juliane M. Ikebara
  • Avishek Adhikari

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Role of cathepsin K in the expression of mechanical hypersensitivity following intra-plantar inflammation

  • Maha Paracha
  • Amit Thakar
  • Travis E. Brown

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Phasic firing of dopaminergic neurons in the ventral tegmental area triggers peripheral immune responses

  • Tasuku Kayama
  • Yuji Ikegaya
  • Takuya Sasaki

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Behavioral and inflammatory sex differences revealed by celecoxib nanotherapeutic treatment of peripheral neuroinflammation

  • Brooke Deal
  • Laura M. Reynolds
  • John A. Pollock

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Distinct spatio-temporal and spectral brain patterns for different thermal stimuli perception

  • Andrei Dragomir
  • Gordon Cheng

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Chondroitin and glucosamine sulphate reduced proinflammatory molecules in the DRG and improved axonal function of injured sciatic nerve of rats

  • Olutayo Folajimi Olaseinde
  • Bamidele Victor Owoyele

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Visualization of learning-induced synaptic plasticity in output neurons of the Drosophila mushroom body γ-lobe

  • Clare E. Hancock
  • Vahid Rostami
  • André Fiala

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Astrocyte-secreted chordin-like 1 regulates spine density after ischemic injury

  • Elena Blanco-Suarez
  • Nicola J. Allen

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Brain structural and functional correlates to defense-related inhibition of muscle sympathetic nerve activity in man

  • Bushra Riaz
  • John J. Eskelin
  • Mikael Elam

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Opto-electrical bimodal recording of neural activity in awake head-restrained mice

  • Luis Fernando Cobar
  • Alireza Kashef
  • Ayumu Tashiro

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Prefrontal projections to the nucleus reuniens signal behavioral relevance of stimuli during associative learning

  • Xiaotian Yu
  • Fasika Jembere
  • Kaori Takehara-Nishiuchi

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Long-lasting reflexive and nonreflexive pain responses in two mouse models of fibromyalgia-like condition

  • Beltrán Álvarez-Pérez
  • Meritxell Deulofeu
  • Pere Boadas-Vaello

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Sexually dimorphic peripheral sensory neurons regulate copulation duration and persistence in male Drosophila

  • Shreyas Jois
  • Yick-Bun Chan
  • Adelaine Kwun-Wai Leung

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

An almost nontoxic tetrodotoxin analog, 5,6,11-trideoxytetrodotoxin, as an odorant for the grass puffer

  • Yoshihisa Noguchi
  • Takehisa Suzuki

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Neuronal modulation in the mouse superior colliculus during covert visual selective attention

  • Lupeng Wang
  • James P. Herman
  • Richard J. Krauzlis

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

An assessment of the rescue action of resveratrol in parkin loss of function-induced oxidative stress in Drosophila melanogaster

  • Adeola O. Adedara
  • Ayoade D. Babalola
  • Amos O. Abolaji

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Wnt/β-catenin signaling promotes neurogenesis in the diencephalospinal dopaminergic system of embryonic zebrafish

  • Markus Westphal
  • Paolo Panza
  • Wolfgang Driever

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

The effects of locomotion on sensory-evoked haemodynamic responses in the cortex of awake mice

  • Clare Howarth

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Ultrafast Doppler imaging and ultrasound localization microscopy reveal the complexity of vascular rearrangement in chronic spinal lesion

  • Benoit Beliard
  • Chaimae Ahmanna
  • Sophie Pezet

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Ultrasound stimulation improves inflammatory resolution, neuroprotection, and functional recovery after spinal cord injury

  • Eun-hee Lee
  • Juyoung Park

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Symptom-based pharmacotherapy for neuropathic pain related to spinal disorders: results from a patient-based assessment

  • Hideaki Nakajima
  • Shuji Watanabe
  • Akihiko Matsumine

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

The role of the meningeal lymphatic system in local meningeal inflammation and trigeminal nociception

  • Nikita Mikhailov
  • Anaïs Virenque
  • Rashid Giniatullin

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Dietary restriction ameliorates TBI-induced phenotypes in Drosophila melanogaster

  • Rebecca Delventhal
  • Emily R. Wooder
  • Mimi M. Shirasu-Hiza

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Odor blocking of stress hormone responses

  • Eun Jeong Lee
  • Luis R. Saraiva
  • Linda B. Buck

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Adenosine A 2A receptors control synaptic remodeling in the adult brain

  • Rui O. Beleza
  • Joana M. Marques

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

The transcription factor unc-130 / FOXD3/4 contributes to the biphasic calcium response required to optimize avoidance behavior

  • Sayaka Hori
  • Shohei Mitani

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Imaging peripheral nerve micro-anatomy with MUSE, 2D and 3D approaches

  • Chaitanya Kolluru
  • Austin Todd
  • David L. Wilson

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

The impact of pitolisant, an H 3 receptor antagonist/inverse agonist, on perirhinal cortex activity in individual neuron and neuronal population levels

  • Kyosuke Hirano
  • Yoshikazu Morishita
  • Hiroshi Nomura

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

The importance of self-efficacy and negative affect for neurofeedback success for central neuropathic pain after a spinal cord injury

  • Krithika Anil
  • Sara Demain
  • Aleksandra Vuckovic

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Disentangling the critical signatures of neural activity

  • Benedetta Mariani
  • Giorgio Nicoletti
  • Samir Suweis

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Neurons in the pigeon visual network discriminate between faces, scrambled faces, and sine grating images

  • William Clark
  • Matthew Chilcott
  • Michael Colombo

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Novel adult cortical neuron processing and screening method illustrates sex- and age-dependent effects of pharmaceutical compounds

  • Arthur Sefiani
  • Cédric G. Geoffroy

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Characterization of somatosensory neuron involvement in the SOD1 G93A mouse model

  • Miguel A. Rubio
  • Mireia Herrando-Grabulosa
  • Xavier Navarro

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Viral strategies for targeting spinal neuronal subtypes in adult wild-type rodents

  • Jaspreet Kaur
  • Rune W. Berg

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Altered activity of pain processing brain regions in association with hip osteoarthritis

  • A. J. Delaney
  • J. N. Powell

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Activation of the Hepcidin-Ferroportin1 pathway in the brain and astrocytic–neuronal crosstalk to counteract iron dyshomeostasis during aging

  • Mariarosa Mezzanotte
  • Giorgia Ammirata
  • Antonella Roetto

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Dynamics of phase oscillator networks with synaptic weight and structural plasticity

  • Kanishk Chauhan
  • Ali Khaledi-Nasab
  • Peter A. Tass

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Oxytocin via oxytocin receptor excites neurons in the endopiriform nucleus of juvenile mice

  • Lindsey M. Biggs
  • Elizabeth A. D. Hammock

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Stria medullaris innervation follows the transcriptomic division of the habenula

  • Iris Juárez-Leal
  • Estefanía Carretero-Rodríguez
  • Eduardo Puelles

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Super-resolved fluorescence imaging of peripheral nerve

  • Iván Coto Hernández
  • Suresh Mohan
  • Nate Jowett

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Internal state affects local neuron function in an early sensory processing center to shape olfactory behavior in Drosophila larvae

  • Seth R. Odell
  • David Clark
  • Dennis Mathew

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Drosophila septin interacting protein 1 regulates neurogenesis in the early developing larval brain

  • Hung-Hsiang Yu

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Contextual memory reactivation modulates Ca 2+ -activity network state in a mushroom body-like center of the crab N. granulata

  • Francisco Javier Maza
  • Francisco José Urbano
  • Alejandro Delorenzi

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Bursting of excitatory cells is linked to interictal epileptic discharge generation in humans

  • Katharina T. Hofer
  • Ágnes Kandrács
  • Lucia Wittner

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Improvement of the affinity of an anti-rat P2X4 receptor antibody by introducing electrostatic interactions

  • Chinatsu Shinozaki
  • Keita Kohno
  • Tadashi Ueda

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

A glibenclamide-sensitive TRPM4-mediated component of CA1 excitatory postsynaptic potentials appears in experimental autoimmune encephalomyelitis

  • Brenna C. Fearey
  • Lars Binkle
  • Christine E. Gee

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Oral cancer induced TRPV1 sensitization is mediated by PAR 2 signaling in primary afferent neurons innervating the cancer microenvironment

  • Nicole N. Scheff
  • Ian M. Wall
  • Brian L. Schmidt

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Vagus nerve stimulation does not improve recovery of forelimb motor or somatosensory function in a model of neuropathic pain

  • Katherine S. Adcock
  • Tanya Danaphongse
  • Seth A. Hays

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Novel roles of phentolamine in protecting axon myelination, muscle atrophy, and functional recovery following nerve injury

  • Zarin Zainul
  • Vijayendran Chandran

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Low frequency visual stimulation enhances slow wave activity without disrupting the sleep pattern in mice

  • Stephen Thankachan
  • Dmitry Gerashchenko

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Neuromodulator-dependent synaptic tagging and capture retroactively controls neural coding in spiking neural networks

  • Andrew B. Lehr
  • Jannik Luboeinski
  • Christian Tetzlaff

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Neural signature of the perceptual decision in the neural population responses of the inferior temporal cortex

  • Mohammad-Reza A. Dehaqani
  • Nazli Emadi
  • Hossein Esteky

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Functional connectivity response to acute pain assessed by fNIRS is associated with BDNF genotype in fibromyalgia: an exploratory study

  • Álvaro de Oliveira Franco
  • Guilherme de Oliveira Venturini
  • Wolnei Caumo

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

All-optical manipulation of the Drosophila olfactory system

  • Mirko Zanon
  • Damiano Zanini
  • Albrecht Haase

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Identifying long-range synaptic inputs using genetically encoded labels and volume electron microscopy

  • Irene P. Ayuso-Jimeno
  • Paolo Ronchi
  • Cornelius T. Gross

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Neuroprotective effects of violacein in a model of inherited amyotrophic lateral sclerosis

  • Silvia Olivera-Bravo
  • Carmen Bolatto
  • Pablo Díaz-Amarilla

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

A miniature fluorescence microscope for multi-plane imaging

  • Giovanni Barbera
  • Da-Ting Lin

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Expression of antioxidant enzymes in lesions of multiple sclerosis and its models

  • Dorsa Moezzi
  • V. Wee Yong

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Passage number affects differentiation of sensory neurons from human induced pluripotent stem cells

  • Erica L. Cantor
  • Bryan P. Schneider

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

A closer look at sex pheromone autodetection in the Oriental fruit moth

  • Alicia Pérez-Aparicio
  • Byrappa Ammagarahalli
  • César Gemeno

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

The dynamics of protein localisation to restricted zones within Drosophila mechanosensory cilia

  • Wangchu Xiang
  • Petra zur Lage
  • Andrew P. Jarman

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Integrative multiomic analyses of dorsal root ganglia in diabetic neuropathic pain using proteomics, phospho-proteomics, and metabolomics

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

LXR agonist modifies neuronal lipid homeostasis and decreases PGD2 in the dorsal root ganglia in western diet-fed mice

  • Nadia Elshareif
  • Chaitanya K. Gavini
  • Virginie Mansuy-Aubert

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Characteristics and stability of sensorimotor activity driven by isolated-muscle group activation in a human with tetraplegia

  • Robert W. Nickl
  • Manuel A. Anaya
  • Pablo A. Celnik

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

A realistic morpho-anatomical connection strategy for modelling full-scale point-neuron microcircuits

  • Daniela Gandolfi
  • Jonathan Mapelli
  • Michele Migliore

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Selective modulation of cortical population dynamics during neuroprosthetic skill learning

  • Ellen L. Zippi
  • Albert K. You
  • Jose M. Carmena

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Post-stroke treatment with argon preserved neurons and attenuated microglia/macrophage activation long-termly in a rat model of transient middle cerebral artery occlusion (tMCAO)

  • Jingjin Liu
  • Michael Veldeman
  • Mark Coburn

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Blast-induced axonal degeneration in the rat cerebellum in the absence of head movement

  • Robin Bishop
  • Seok Joon Won
  • Raymond A. Swanson

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Cellular mechanisms underlying state-dependent neural inhibition with magnetic stimulation

  • Vincent Chen
  • Jenna Hendee

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Peripheral N -methyl- d -aspartate receptor activation contributes to monosodium glutamate-induced headache but not nausea behaviours in rats

  • Tarique Benbow
  • Felisha Teja
  • Brian E. Cairns

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Ferroptosis inhibition by deferiprone, attenuates myelin damage and promotes neuroprotection in demyelinated optic nerve

  • Atefeh Rayatpour
  • Forough Foolad
  • Mohammad Javan

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

FABP5 deletion in nociceptors augments endocannabinoid signaling and suppresses TRPV1 sensitization and inflammatory pain

  • Diane M. Bogdan
  • Keith Studholme
  • Martin Kaczocha

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Conditioned up and down modulations of short latency gamma band oscillations in visual cortex during fear learning in humans

  • Alejandro Santos-Mayo
  • Javier de Echegaray
  • Stephan Moratti

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

Spontaneous activity in whisker-innervating region of neonatal mouse trigeminal ganglion

  • Piu Banerjee
  • Takuji Iwasato

Quick links

  • Explore articles by subject
  • Guide to authors
  • Editorial policies

neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

IMAGES

  1. 115 Interesting Neuroscience Topics For Research Papers

    neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

  2. 120 Neuroscience Research Topics: Explore the World

    neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

  3. Neuroscience Research Topics : 100+ Cool Ideas

    neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

  4. 143 Neuroscience Research Topics For Fantastic Results

    neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

  5. 202 Interesting Neuroscience Topics For Students To Write About

    neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

  6. 150 Best Neuroscience Research Topics and Ideas for Students

    neuroscience topics for research paper 2020

VIDEO

  1. Curiosities and Breakthroughs: Neuroscience News Top 5 of the Week

  2. 2020 Top 5 Recap: Neuroscience

  3. Pulling ideas from the brain

  4. Brain Beats: Top Neuroscience News Discoveries of the Week

  5. How to find research topics

  6. From Brain Cancer to Stroke, Dementia, and Autism

COMMENTS

  1. Top 100 in Neuroscience

    This collection highlights our most downloaded* neuroscience papers published in 2020. Featuring authors from around the world, these papers showcase valuable research from an international community.

  2. Research articles

    Read the latest Research articles from Nature Neuroscience. ... 2020 (137) 2019 (185) 2018 (144) ... This paper shows that compulsive-like grooming in Sapap3-knockout mice can be reduced by closed ...

  3. Frontiers in Neuroscience

    Challenges and Current Research Status of Vertigo/Vestibular Diseases Volume III. Sulin Zhang. Andrea Castellucci. Hubertus Axer. Nicolas Perez-Fernandez. 362 views. Part of the most cited neuroscience journal series which explores the brain - from the new eras of causation and anatomical neurosciences to neuroeconomics and neuroenergetics.

  4. Journal of Neuroscience Research

    The Journal of Neuroscience Research ( JNR) publishes pioneering research relevant to the development, function, and pathophysiology of the nervous system. Molecular, cellular, systems, and translational approaches are all considered. Papers explore basic research and clinical aspects of neurology, neuropathology, psychiatry, or psychology.

  5. Frontiers in Neuroscience

    Part of the most cited neuroscience journal series which explores the brain - from the new eras of causation and anatomical neurosciences to neuroeconomics and neuroenergetics. ... Volume 14 - 2020 Research Topics See all (1,814) Learn more about Research Topics. Footer. Guidelines. Author guidelines; Editor guidelines; Policies and publication ...

  6. Top 100 in Neuroscience

    Top 100 in Neuroscience | Scientific Reports. The Scientific Reports team is pleased to announce the most read* articles from 2021 in Neuroscience. Featuring authors from around the world, these papers highlight valuable research from an international community. Published in Social Sciences. Apr 11, 2022.

  7. Topics and trends in artificial intelligence assisted human brain research

    Recent reviews on AI-enhanced neuroscience research and its relevant topics. Reviewer(s) and year Research topic No. of articles Method ... We first created the dataset for analysis by extracting the research papers from the Science Citation Index Extended (SCIE) and Social Science Citation Index ... 2020:103855 10.1016/j.compedu.2020.103855 ...

  8. Neuroscience Research Topics & Ideas (Includes Free Webinar)

    Neuroscience Research Ideas (Continued) The impact of chronic pain on brain structure and connectivity. Analyzing the effects of physical exercise on neurogenesis and cognitive aging. The neural mechanisms underlying hallucinations in psychiatric and neurological disorders. Investigating the impact of music therapy on brain recovery post-stroke.

  9. Research Articles

    CCK+ interneurons contribute to thalamus-evoked feed-forward inhibition in prelimbic prefrontal cortex

  10. Recent advances in neurotechnologies with broad potential for ...

    Recent research establishes the foundations for a broad range of creative neurotechnologies that enable unique modes of operation in this context. This review focuses on those systems with proven utility in animal model studies and with levels of technical maturity that suggest a potential for broad deployment to the neuroscience community in ...

  11. Top 100 in Neuroscience

    Top 100 in Neuroscience. This collection highlights our most downloaded* neuroscience papers published in 2021. Featuring authors from around the world, these papers showcase valuable research ...

  12. The Next 50 Years of Neuroscience

    On the 50th anniversary of the Society for Neuroscience, we reflect on the remarkable progress that the field has made in understanding the nervous system, and look forward to the contributions of the next 50 years. We predict a substantial acceleration of our understanding of the nervous system that will drive the development of new therapeutic strategies to treat diseases over the course of ...

  13. Attention in Psychology, Neuroscience, and Machine Learning

    Attention is the important ability to flexibly control limited computational resources. It has been studied in conjunction with many other topics in neuroscience and psychology including awareness, vigilance, saliency, executive control, and learning. It has also recently been applied in several domains in machine learning. The relationship between the study of biological attention and its use ...

  14. Visualizing Research Trends on Culture Neuroscience (2008-2021): A

    A bibliometric analysis was performed on 113 articles in the field of cultural neuroscience from 2008 to 2021 using data from the core collection of Web of Science. Network visualization software VOSviewer and ITGInsight were used for performance analysis and science mapping. Specifically, the performance analysis included countries ...

  15. Neuroscience Research

    Neuroscience Research is an international journal for high quality articles in all branches of neuroscience, from the molecular to the behavioral levels. The journal is published in collaboration with the Japan Neuroscience Society and is open to all contributors in the world. Neuroscience Research publishes original full-length research ...

  16. 202 Neuroscience Topics To Write About In Depth

    Here, is a compilation of some of the interesting topics in the field of neuroscience. Brainstorming and its health benefits to the human brain. Understanding the role of the brain and its reception to emotions. A case study of how different music tones are received in the brain. A look into how PTSD is developed in the human brain.

  17. Roles and Research Trends of Neuroscience on Major Information Systems

    A total of 99 neuroscience papers and their citation data were included from 19 major information systems journals of SCI/SSCI. This study analyzed bibliometric data from 2010 to 2021 to identify the most productive countries, universities, authors, journals, and prolific publications in NeuroIS. ... In terms of bibliometric research, Su et al ...

  18. 121 Neuroscience Research Topics (Fresh for 2023)

    This means you need to choose one of our current topics in neuroscience: Cerebellar Neurons that can help you lose weight. Effects of a meat-based diet. Latest brain mapping technology. CT scans in 2023. Brain implants that can control a computer. An in-depth look at super-agers.

  19. Blog

    Lets take a look at 9 of the top neuroscience discoveries of this past year. 1. Live 3D Brain Function Mapping. Earlier this year MIT scientists developed a new technique to pair structural mapping (brain anatomy) with functional mapping (how the brain behaves) - the first time this has been properly achieved. In addition, this has been done in ...

  20. Top 100 in Neuroscience

    Top 100 in Neuroscience. This collection highlights our most downloaded* neuroscience papers published in 2019. Featuring authors from around the world, these papers feature valuable research from ...

  21. Top 100 in Neuroscience

    Top 100 in Neuroscience - 2022. This collection highlights our most downloaded* neuroscience papers published in 2022. Featuring authors from around the world, these papers showcase valuable ...

  22. 2024 AP Exam Dates

    AP Seminar and AP Research students to submit performance tasks as final and their presentations to be scored by their AP Seminar or AP Research teachers. AP Computer Science Principles students to submit their Create performance task as final. Late Testing . Occasionally, circumstances make it necessary for students to test late.

  23. Accessing Neuromarketing Scientific Performance: Research Gaps and

    1. Introduction. As a research field, neuromarketing emerged from the application of neuroscience methods and techniques for marketing purposes, i.e., consumer behavior as a response to a certain stimulus [].In this context, neuromarketing is an interdisciplinary domain with great of potential, as it allows researchers to understand and predict consumer choices and behavior.