Elephant in the Lab

Cornelia van Scherpenberg, Lindsey Bultema, Anja Jahn, Michaela Löffler, Vera Minneker, Jana Lasser

Manifestations of power abuse in academia and how to prevent them

11 March 2021 | doi:10.5281/zenodo.4580544 | 1 Comment

Manifestations of power abuse in academia and how to prevent them

A group of researchers from the German N² network presents the results of a survey among PhD students on the abuse of power in science and outlines ways to counter it

This article results from a cooperation within  N² , a network of the  Helmholtz Juniors ,  Leibniz PhD Network  and  Max Planck PhDnet . The International PhD Program Mainz (IPP) is an associated member of N². Representing more than 16.000 doctoral researchers, it is the biggest network of doctoral researchers in Germany. The N² member networks regularly conduct surveys and, individually or in the framework of N², represent issues concerning the doctoral researchers of their networks towards the respective research organizations as well as to the public.

research paper on abuse of power

In recent years, evidence of cases of power abuse in academia have been made public in various news reports about poor leadership, bullying and even sexual harassment of employees by superiors at universities (Boytchev, 2020; Hartocollis, 2018; Schenkel, 2018) and non-university research organisations in Germany and abroad (Devlin & Marsh, 2018; Weber, 2018). In addition, surveys amongst employees (especially doctoral researchers) at these institutions has revealed alarming numbers of instances of (witnessing) bullying (In German reports of abusive behaviour at the workplace, the word „mobbing“ is often used for „bullying“), discrimination or sexual harassment by a superior (e.g., Arcudi et al., 2019; Beadle et al., 2020; Olsthoorn et al., 2020; Peukert et al., 2020; Regler et al., 2019; Schraudner et al., 2019).

These numbers and reports suggest that the basis for these incidents is inherent in the structures of the academic system. The academic system, not only in Germany, is set up very hierarchically – people with an established career (e.g., a professor, a research group leader, or a director of a research institute) sit at the top, while early career researchers, continuing their education and advancing their career, are at the lower end of the hierarchy.

In this article we outline the manifestations of power hierarchy and dependencies in the academic system from the point of view of doctoral researchers (DRs) – based on findings from surveys on the situation of DRs at non-university research organisations in Germany, as well as on our own experiences as DRs and elected representatives of the DRs in the Helmholtz Association, Leibniz Association, Max Planck Society and IPP Mainz, respectively. Moreover, we give recommendations on how to navigate and prevent instances of power abuse and summarize which measures have already been taken by research institutions such as those with which N² member networks are affiliated (A detailed report on our results and observations can also be found in the contribution to the 2020 conference “Absender unbekannt. Anonyme Anschuldigungen in der Wissenschaft“ (Lasser et al., accepted)).

DATA COLLECTION ON SUPERVISION AND INSTANCES OF POWER ABUSE IN THE 2019 N² HARMONIZED SURVEY

In 2019, the N² members conducted harmonized surveys in their networks. The individual survey reports (Beadle et al., 2020; Olsthoorn et al., 2020; Peukert et al., 2020) shed light on the relationship between supervision, working conditions, mental health and experiences of abusive behaviour. Supplemented by reports from other large national (Briedis et al., 2018; Schraudner et al., 2019) and international surveys (Wellcome Trust, 2020; Woolston, 2019), they serve as evidence for the status quo of the prevalence of power abuse in (German) academia, which we outline below.

Prevalence of conflict cases amongst early career researchers According to our surveys 10-13% of DRs report to have been subjected to bullying by a superior at least once (Figure 1A). Only one third of those who have experienced bullying reported the incident to an official body (Figure 1B) and out of those, only about a quarter are satisfied with how the matter was resolved (Figure 1C). In other surveys (e.g., Schraudner et al., 2019), the respondents’ two most important reasons for not reporting an incident are their concerns that there would be no consequences for the perpetrator and the fear that their career would be impeded. Indeed, significant numbers reported general negative consequences or specific negative consequences for their career after they made a report.

research paper on abuse of power

Figure 1: Experiences with bullying and conflict reporting. A: Participants were asked “While working at your institute/center, have you at any point been subjected to bullying by a superior?”. The graph shows the fraction over all respondents who have at least once experienced bullying by a superior. B: Participants were asked “Did you ever report a conflict with a superior?”. The graph shows the fraction over all respondents who answered “yes”. C: For those who answered yes in (B), participants were asked “Please indicate the level of satisfaction with the consequences of your report?”. The answers were given on a scale from “very dissatisfied” to “very satisfied” and are grouped here (dissatisfied = very dissatisfied + dissatisfied, neither nor, satisfied = very satisfied + satisfied). Numbers compiled from the individual survey reports by the respective networks that are part of N2 (Beadle et al., 2020; Olsthoorn et al., 2020; Peukert et al., 2020). Graphic created by Theresa Kuhl, Helmholtz Zentrum München – German Research Centre for Environmental Health.

Amongst the DRs represented by N², about a quarter of DRs are dissatisfied with their supervision (Figure 2A) and about a third consider giving up their PhD “often” or “occasionally”, a tendency that correlates with experiences of bullying (Figure 2B, see also Briedis et al, 2018). These findings universally show that DRs perceive the strong and multiple dependencies between them and their supervisors as problematic, leading to dissatisfaction, mental health problems (For a detailed report on the mental health crisis among doctoral researchers see our previous post on this blog) and thoughts about quitting.

research paper on abuse of power

Figure 2: Satisfaction with supervision and aspects of academia. A: Participants were asked “How satisfied are you with your PhD supervision in general?”. The answers were given on a scale from “very dissatisfied” to “very satisfied” and are grouped here (dissatisfied = very dissatisfied + dissatisfied + rather dissatisfied, satisfied = very satisfied + satisfied + rather dissatisfied). Numbers compiled from the individual survey reports by the respective networks that are part of N2 (Beadle et al., 2020; Olsthoorn et al., 2020; Peukert et al., 2020). Graphic created by Theresa Kuhl, Helmholtz Zentrum München – German Research Centre for Environmental Health.

Multiple dependencies in academia 

Why do conflicts between supervisors and DRs occur and how do they manifest?  Due to the academic hierarchy, power differentials between DRs and their superiors exist in different, often interconnected areas of academic life. In the majority of cases, the employer and scientific supervisor are the same person, who therefore has administrative power over future employment and contract extensions as well as over evaluation of scientific results. These professors, institute directors, research group leaders, or principal investigators (PIs) and their relationship to the DR have a direct impact on the DR’s academic reputation and future successful navigation through the academic system (e.g., as co-authors on publications and grant applications, through grading of theses, or through recommendation letters). These various dependencies on the superior put the DR in a vulnerable position. They have a lot to lose and may therefore be unable or unwilling to defend themselves in cases of conflict. A closer look at these characteristics of power differentials reveals their potential as sources of conflicts and abusive behavior – but also the opportunity to alleviate them through measures and systematic changes.

Power through scientific evaluation Traditionally, in German universities and research institutions, the main supervisor of a dissertation is also the main evaluator of the project. This means that they have to assess the quality of a thesis which results from work they supervised themselves. This conflation between supervision and assessment often results in a conflict of interest that can take many forms.

On the one hand, there are no incentives for superiors to truthfully evaluate the work they have supervised (sometimes on a daily basis): Senior researchers (such as PIs or professors) are often assessed on the number of DRs they graduate – an incentive to let people pass with low grades rather than failing them due to substandard work.

On the other hand and more commonly, supervisors can use the threat of a bad thesis evaluation to compel work from the DRs beyond what has been contractually agreed upon – for example when it comes to scientific publications. The duration required to publish in an academic journal varies greatly, depending on the journal and field. Although the researcher can assess the average time it takes from submission to acceptance for a specific journal, once the article is submitted, they no longer have control over how quickly the submission progresses through the peer review system (Huisman & Smits, 2017). In turn, this can delay the thesis completion by many months.

research paper on abuse of power

Figure 3: Employment situation and working hours of DRs in the N2 member organizations. Numbers compiled from the individual survey reports by the respective networks that are part of N2 (Beadle et al., 2020; Olsthoorn et al., 2020; Peukert et al., 2020). Graphic created by Theresa Kuhl, Helmholtz Zentrum München – German Research Centre for Environmental Health.

Power over type and length of employment Researchers usually have a high intrinsic motivation to work extensively on their research, and this is exacerbated by the high pressure and competitive environment in academia (Woolston, 2019). However, pressure from supervisors is given as a main reason for long average working hours (see Figure 3A), work on weekends and a tendency not to take vacation time in academia. This pressure is intensified by the precarious employment situation of the majority of DRs.

Employment contracts for DRs are almost always time-limited: 98% of DRs working at universities or non-university research institutions have fixed-term contracts (BUWIN, 2021, p. 111); for postdocs or scientists without tenure under the age of 45 this number still ranges between 84-96% (BUWIN, 2021, p.112). Data reveals that a PhD in Germany takes between 3.5 and 7.1 years, depending on the subject (BUWIN, 2021, p. 138; Jaksztat et al., 2012; and see Figure 3B). However, our survey results show that many DRs receive a contract with a duration of less than 36 months (Figure 3C). These contract durations are not long enough to ensure the completion of a PhD within the first employment contract and make it very likely that DRs will need at least one contract extension to complete their degree. The situation is even more precarious for other early career researchers such as postdocs and junior group leaders. In 2017, half of all employment contracts were reported to be limited to less than one year (BUWIN, 2017, p.132), even though research projects regularly take substantially longer to be completed. This situation seems to have improved slightly, with average contract durations of 28 months for postdocs (BUWIN, 2021, p. 116/117).

Decisions about further employment of DRs are often taken by a single person who has power over the research unit, institute or third-party funding on which the DR is employed. This means that DRs regularly depend on them to extend their contracts. While a permanent contract cannot be easily terminated as it usually requires justification and/or involvement of the works or staff council, employees with short-term contracts can be easily let go by simply not renewing their contract. In addition to increasing the researchers’ financial vulnerability, these precarious employment conditions are especially threatening to international researchers from outside the European Union, who might lose their residence permit, which is directly tied to their working contract.

Power over reputation and knowledge transfer Particularly at the beginning of an academic career, DRs are dependent on their superiors to share their knowledge and experience of the academic sphere with them, in order to be successful in academia. For example, the PI or professor is much more knowledgeable about appropriate funding agencies, appropriate venues for publications, conferences and the scientific network.

Researchers profit significantly when their supervisors introduce them into relevant academic circles and help them move through the bottleneck of academic hierarchy in general. This puts supervisors in a unique position of power, to foster or damage the reputation of the DRs. Less than half of the DRs report that they receive the support they need from their supervisors to further their career. This can manifest itself in supervisors allowing or preventing DRs from attending conferences and criticizing their graduates in front of other senior researchers and potential future employers. The most formal shape that this power differential takes is the letter of recommendation which is required for almost all applications to future academic and non-academic positions. Supervisors have no obligation to provide such a letter and if they do, they have no obligation to substantiate a negative assessment of the DR. Moreover, in most academic fields, supervisors have the power to decide about authorship contributions in academic publications. In many cases, at least one first or corresponding author publication is required to receive a doctorate and to successfully apply for a research position or grant after completion of the PhD. The pressure to “publish or perish” leaves DRs dependent on their supervisor to grant them this (first) authorship (Wu et al., 2019).

Lastly, it is much harder for international researchers (especially on short-term contracts) to familiarize themselves with the bureaucratic structures, workplace regulations and their own employment rights.

THE VICIOUS CYCLE OF ABUSIVE BEHAVIOUR IN ACADEMIA…

As outlined above, power differentials exist in many aspects of academia and DRs depend on their supervisors for their livelihood, reputation and future career. These systemic conditions lay the ground for ubiquitous potential conflicts. However, as incidents of abusive behaviour are rarely reported, the academic system is deprived of much-needed feedback from a majority of its members, hindering improvement. At worst, DRs might pass on their own experiences of bad and abusive supervision when they become postdocs or when it is their turn to supervise and lead a research group – the vicious cycle of bad scientific leadership and dependencies. This situation is damaging for the academic system as a whole, as it rewards conformism and alienates original thinkers and creative young minds from research. This way, the competitiveness of academia is reproduced time and time again, leaving researchers little room to pursue their original goal: to advance scientific progress (see also Dirnagl, 2021).

Therefore, we propose a variety of measures that have the potential not only to resolve but prevent conflicts related to power differentials and break the vicious cycle. The recommendations are based on a  position paper  on Power Abuse and Conflict Resolution published by our network (N², 2019). We would like to stress that some recommendations we describe here are already (being) implemented in research institutions, also through fruitful exchanges between the institutions and PhD representatives or networks (see Infobox).

Conflict Resolution & Prevention of Power Abuse  Measures implemented by the Helmholtz Association (HGF), the Max Planck Society (MPG) & the Leibniz Association (WGL). Many research institutions and universities have implemented measures for prevention and resolution of conflict. In many instances, these measures were the result of a fruitful collaboration with the respective PhD networks. The following measures have been implemented in the organisations represented by N²: 

Supervision agreement and Thesis Advisory Committees

  • 55-70% of DRs have a supervision agreement and/or a Thesis Advisory Committee (N² harmonized survey 2019).
  • Supervision agreement composed by Leibniz PhD Network in 2020, already implemented in some WGL institutes ( link ).
  • Supervision and employment guidelines on an institutional level are stated in doctoral guidelines ( HGF , MPG ,  WGL )

Leadership trainings

  • MPG started seminars and coaching for directors and research group leaders within the framework of the  Planck Academy  founded in 2020.
  • The HGF has introduced the  Helmholtz Leadership Academy .
  • The WGL has established a lecture program on leadership ( Führungskolleg ).

Conflict reporting and resolution

  • In order to deal with conflicts, institutes and centers of the HGF, MPG and WGL have local ombudspersons.
  • The WGL introduced a central ombuds committee in 2020 ( link ).
  • The HGF is currently establishing a central ombudsperson with the new good scientific practice guidelines.
  • The MPG assigned an external law firm as an independent contact point in 2018, and in 2019 established an Internal Investigations Unit as a central reporting office that can initiate investigations.
  • The WGL created the external Advice Center for Conflict Guidance and Prevention.

… AND RECOMMENDATIONS FOR HOW TO BREAK IT

Supervision agreements First of all, power differentials can be navigated and power abuse prevented if DRs and their supervisors sign and adhere to a supervision agreement, which clearly outlines roles, expectations and procedures from both sides. Such an agreement can go a long way in preventing a mismatch of expectations or misunderstandings right from the beginning of a project and side-stepping potential conflicts that could escalate. They also establish a level of accountability for the supervision provided, and also for the work expected from the DR.

Thesis Advisory Committees Moreover, PhD supervision should be spread out to more than one person, ideally in the form of a Thesis Advisory Committee, which includes external, independent advisors who consult on the progress of the dissertation project. This is already common practice in the UK, but also in most institutes belonging to the Max Planck Society (see infobox).

At the same time, when it comes to thesis assessment, there should be a clear division between main supervisor and main referee. At the minimum, as suggested also by the Hochschulrektorenkonferenz (HRK, 2012), external reviewers should be included in the thesis evaluation and grading process.

Leadership and communication trainings An additional measure which promises to alleviate the instances of power differentials outlined above, is to introduce leadership training for any senior researcher taking on supervision and/or leadership responsibility. This addresses the assumption that power abuse often does not happen out of spite but is rather an expression of the inability to effectively manage and lead a research group. With the latest incidents of power abuse (Boytchev, 2020; Devlin & Marsh, 2018; Hartocollis, 2018; Weber, 2018), it has become once more apparent that scientific excellence does not equal good leadership skills. Therefore, any researcher who is responsible for the supervision of DRs should take part in mandatory and regular leadership training in order to learn how best to guide the DRs and their other employees through the existing differentials in reputation, knowledge and employment status. At the same time, doctoral researchers and postdocs would benefit from training courses focusing on communication and cultural skills, empowerment for networking and career choices, and knowledge about navigating the academic system and its obstacles.

Confidential and independent conflict resolution Whenever a conflict occurs, the victim should have the opportunity to make a confidential report of the incident with the expectation of a timely investigation. Therefore, it is important that the existing reporting structures are clearly outlined by the institution and regularly communicated to its members. Conflict resolution should involve a multi-staged investigation, as appropriate to the level of escalation of the conflict. Such a process should start at local conflict resolution mechanisms (Ombudsperson, works council etc.) and include the possibility to involve investigation of the conflict case by an independent committee. All steps of the conflict resolution mechanism must be clearly documented and communicated to all parties involved. In all instances, when the victim feels it is necessary, their anonymity must be protected. The trust in these mechanisms is key to establish them as a feedback structure for researchers from all levels.

Protection through anonymity From our own experience as representatives of DRs and through studying the data from our surveys, we know that a strong motivator for DRs not to report incidents of abusive behaviour is the fear of negative consequences for their careers. Thus, the possibility to anonymously contact people responsible for conflict resolution (such as an ombudsperson or gender equality officer) allows victims of power abuse to receive support without the fear of direct repercussions from their supervisor. Importantly, the identity of the person reporting a conflict needs to remain anonymous to the offender, even after the conclusion of the investigation, to protect the reputation and career of the victim.

Recently, anonymous accusations in the academic context have been discussed critically (e.g., Buchhorn & Freisinger, 2020). We acknowledge that allegations of power abuse can have a very damaging impact on the reputation and career of senior researchers as well. However, firstly, according to a recent report by the German “Ombudsman für die Wissenschaft” (Czesnick & Rixen, in press), anonymous accusations only account for approximately 10% of all accusations. Secondly, assessing the substance of an accusation can be achieved without knowing the identity of the accuser. Lastly, anonymity extends to the potential offender as well, ensuring their opportunity to defend themselves and resolve the incident without damage to their reputation.

Overall, anonymity is an absolute necessity to alleviate the power differentials between DRs and senior researchers and encourage reporting of incidents and corrective action. Taking into account the measures which have already been implemented (see Infobox), the developments are promising. As representatives of doctoral researchers, we regularly communicate our recommendations to various stakeholders of the academic system within our organizations and beyond and highly appreciate when they are taken seriously and when PhD representatives are invited to take part in the development of appropriate measures. We recognize the importance of and the need to change the organizational culture surrounding power differentials in academia. While a system is hard to change, we believe that implementation of the measures outlined above will reduce the incidences of abusive behavior and enable the academic system to deal with conflicts that it currently cannot resolve in a satisfactory manner. Only through collaboration on all levels of the academic hierarchy can we maintain a system that provides a healthy feedback culture, offers a supportive and sustainable working environment, and fosters excellent research.

Arcudi, A., Cumurovic, A., Gotter, C., Graeber, D., Joly, P., Ott, V., Schanze, J.-L., Thater, S., Weltin, M., & Yenikent, S. (2019). Doctoral Researchers in the Leibniz Association: Final Report of the 2017 Leibniz PhD Survey .

Beadle, B., Do, S., El Youssoufi, D., Felder, D., Gorenflos López, J., Jahn, A., Pérez-Bosch Quesada, E., Rottleb, T., Rüter, F., Schanze, J.-L., Stroppe, A.-K., Thater, S., Verrière, A., & Weltin, M. (2020). Being a Doctoral Researcher in the Leibniz Association: 2019 Leibniz PhD Network Survey Report . Leibniz PhD Network. https://www.ssoar.info/ssoar/handle/document/69403

Boytchev, H. (2020). Neue Vorwürfe von Mobbing und Willkür in der Max-Planck-Gesellschaft. BuzzFeed .

Briedis, K., Lietz, A., Ruß, U., Schwabe, U., Weber, A., Birkelbach, R., & Hoffstätter, U. (2018). NACAPS 2018 .

Buchhorn, E., & Freisinger, G. M. (2020). Mission Rufmord: Mobbing gegen Führungskräfte. manager magazin .

BUWIN (2017). Bundesbericht Wissenschaftlicher Nachwuchs 2017: Statistische Daten und Forschungsbefunde zu Promovierenden und Promovierten in Deutschland. http://www.oapen.org/download/?type=document&docid=640942

BUWIN (2021). Bundesbericht Wissenschaftlicher Nachwuchs 2021: Statistische Daten und Forschungsbefunde zu Promovierenden und Promovierten in Deutschland. https://www.buwin.de/dateien/buwin-2021.pdf

Czesnick, H., & Rixen, S. (in press). Sind anonyme Hinweise auf wissenschaftliches Fehlverhalten ein Problem? Eine Einschätzung aus Sicht des “Ombudsman für die Wissenschaft.”

Devlin, H., & Marsh, S. (2018). Top cancer scientist loses £3.5m of funding after bullying claims . https://www.theguardian.com/science/2018/aug/17/top-cancer-scientist-has-35m-in-grants-revoked-after-bullying-claims

Dirnagl, U. (2021). Back to the Future: Von industrieller zu inhaltlicher Forschungsbewertung [Laborjournal Online]. Einsichten Eines Wissenschaftsnarren. https://www.laborjournal.de/rubric/narr/narr/n_21_01.php

Hartocollis, A. (2018, November 15). Dartmouth Professors Are Accused of Sexual Abuse by 7 Women in Lawsuit (Published 2018). The New York Times . https://www.nytimes.com/2018/11/15/us/dartmouth-professors-sexual-harassment.html

HRK. (2012). Empfehlung zur Qualitätssicherung in Promotionsverfahren .

Huisman, J., & Smits, J. (2017). Duration and quality of the peer review process: The author’s perspective. Scientometrics , 113 (1), 633–650. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11192-017-2310-5

Jaksztat, S., Preßler, N., & Briedis, K. (2012). Promotions- und Arbeitsbedingungen Promovierender im Vergleich . HIS: Forum Hochschule. https://www.uni-heidelberg.de/md/journal/2013/04/fh201215_promotion.pdf

Lasser, J., Bultema, L., Jahn, A., Löffler, M., Minneker, V., & van Scherpenberg, C. (submitted). Power abuse and anonymous accusation in academia from the point of view of early career researchers. In Anonyme Anschuldigungen in der Wissenschaft .

N². (2019). Power abuse and conflict resolution .

Olsthoorn, L. H. M., Heckmann, L. A., Filippi, A., Vieira, R. M., Varanasi, R. S., Lasser, J., Bäuerle, F., Zeis, P., Schulte-Sasse, R., & Group 2019/2020, M. P. P. survey. (2020). PhDnet Report 2019 . Max Planck PhDnet. https://pure.mpg.de/pubman/faces/ViewItemOverviewPage.jsp?itemId=item_3243876_4

Peukert, K., Jacobi, L., Geuer, J., Paredes Cisneros, I., Löffler, M., Lienig, T., Taylor, S., Gusic, M., Novakovic, N., Kuhl, T., Ordini, E., Runge, A., Samoylov, O., Härtel, M., Amend, A.-L., & Nagel, M. (2020). Survey report 2019 . Helmholtz Juniors. https://www.helmholtz.de/fileadmin/user_upload/06_jobs_talente/Helmholtz-Juniors/Survey_Report2019.pdf

Regler, B., Einhorn, L., Lasser, J., Vögele, M., Elizarova, S., Bäuerle, F., Wu, C., Förste, S., Shenolikar, J., & Group 2018, P. S. (2019). PhDnet Report 2018 . https://doi.org/10.17617/2.3052826

Schenkel, L. (2018). Die ETH entlässt Professorin Carollo wegen Mobbing-Vorwürfen – 7 Antworten zur ersten Kündigung in der Geschichte der Hochschule . Neue Zürcher Zeitung. https://www.nzz.ch/zuerich/eth-zuerich-entlaesst-professorin-carollo-die-wichtigsten-fakten-ld.1495950

Schraudner, M., Striebing, C., & Hochfeld, K. (2019). Work culture and work atmosphere in the Max Planck Society . Fraunhofer Center for Responsible Research and Innovation. https://www.mpg.de/14284109/mpg-arbeitskultur-ergebnisbericht-englisch.pdf

Weber, C. (2018). Max-Planck-Direktorin Tania Singer verlässt Institut . Süddeutsche.de. https://www.sueddeutsche.de/wissen/max-planck-empathie-singer-1.4240504

Wellcome Trust. (2020). What Researchers Think About the Culture They Work In . Wellcome Trust. https://wellcome.org/sites/default/files/what-researchers-think-about-the-culture-they-work-in.pdf

Woolston, C. (2019). PhDs: The tortuous truth. Nature , 575 (7782), 403–406. https://doi.org/10.1038/d41586-019-03459-7

Wu, C. M., Regler, B., Bäuerle, F. K., Vögele, M., Einhorn, L., Elizarova, S., Förste, S., Shenolikar, J., & Lasser, J. (2019). Perceptions of publication pressure in the Max Planck Society. Nature Human Behaviour , 3 (10), 1029–1030. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-019-0728-x

zidane

thank you for the article

Submit a Comment Cancel reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

First we need to confirm, that you're human. fifty four ÷ = nine

Author info

Cornelia van Scherpenberg is a doctoral researcher at the Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Science where she researches the neural basis of language production processes. She is a member of the N² advisory board and was deputy spokesperson of the Max Planck PhDnet and N² board member in 2020.

Lindsey Bultema is a doctoral researcher at the Max Planck Institute for the Structure and Dynamics of Matter, Hamburg. She’s focusing on in-liquid electron imaging techniques. She is the 2020 Spokesperson of the Max Planck PhDnet and current advisory board member of N².

Anja Jahn is a doctoral researcher at the Leibniz Institute for History and Culture of Eastern Europe (GWZO) in Leipzig. In research she focuses on multilingualism in the literatures of East Central Europe. She is member of the N² Advisory board and was spokesperson of the Leibniz PhD Network and N² Board member from September 2019 to October 2020.

Michaela Löffler is researching hydrogen isotope effects at the Helmholtz Centre for Environmental Research – UFZ in Leipzig. She was spokesperson for the doctoral representation Helmholtz Juniors and thus N² Board member in 2020.

Vera Minneker is a doctoral researcher and coordinator of the “International PhD Programme on Gene Regulation, Epigenetics and Genome Stability“ (IPP) at the Institute for Molecular Biology gGmbH Mainz. She was an elected doctoral researcher representative of the IPP and board member of N² from April 2019 to August 2020. She studies the formation of oncogenic chromosome translocations using high-throughput imaging and automated image analysis.

Jana Lasser is a PostDoc at the Complexity Science Hub Vienna and the Medical University Vienna. She works on modelling complex social systems with a focus on the connection between social & societal dynamics and mental & physical health. She is a member of the N² advisory board and was spokesperson of the Max Planck PhDnet in 2018.

On power and its corrupting effects: the effects of power on human behavior and the limits of accountability systems

Affiliation.

  • 1 Independent Scholar, Yardley, PA, USA.
  • PMID: 37645621
  • PMCID: PMC10461512
  • DOI: 10.1080/19420889.2023.2246793

Power is an all-pervasive, and fundamental force in human relationships and plays a valuable role in social, political, and economic interactions. Power differences are important in social groups in enhancing group functioning. Most people want to have power and there are many benefits to having power. However, power is a corrupting force and this has been a topic of interest for centuries to scholars from Plato to Lord Acton. Even with increased knowledge of power's corrupting effect and safeguards put in place to counteract such tendencies, power abuse remains rampant in society suggesting that the full extent of this effect is not well understood. In this paper, an effort is made to improve understanding of power's corrupting effects on human behavior through an integrated and comprehensive synthesis of the neurological, sociological, physiological, and psychological literature on power. The structural limits of justice systems' capability to hold powerful people accountable are also discussed.

Keywords: Dominance; Dominance Hierarchy; Justice systems; Power and Access; Power and Evolution; Power and Mental Health; Power and Nepotism; Power and Sex; Power and Status; Power and self-interested behavior; Power and size; Powerlessness and behavior; Social Rank; Social Status; Socioeconomic status; high power and low status; power; power addiction; power and aggressive behavior; power and ambition; power and bariatric surgery; power and bias; power and cooperation; power and corruption; power and credibility; power and dehumanizing behavior; power and demeaning behavior; power and disinhibited behavior; power and entitlement; power and gossip; power and hypocrisy; power and overconfidence; power and physical attractiveness; power and self righteousness; power and sexual harassment; power and unethical behavior; power and victimhood; structural limits of accountability systems.

© 2023 The Author(s). Published by Informa UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis Group.

Publication types

Grants and funding.

Academia.edu no longer supports Internet Explorer.

To browse Academia.edu and the wider internet faster and more securely, please take a few seconds to  upgrade your browser .

Enter the email address you signed up with and we'll email you a reset link.

  • We're Hiring!
  • Help Center

paper cover thumbnail

Abuse of Power depicted in Animal Farm Research Paper

Profile image of Senanur Kir

Related Papers

English Literature and Language Review

Dr. OMAR O JABAK

The present research study attempted to provide an interpretation of George Orwell"s Animal Farm as an outcry against false revolutionary leaders who go back on their promises and turn into dehumanized dictators even worse than the dictators against whom they and their fellow revolutionaries rebelled. To achieve that objective, the researcher read the novella critically within its socio-political context and traced the transformation of the leading character, Napoleon, who stands for such revolutionary leaders. The data of the current research were all extracted from Orwell"s Animal Farm. The researcher used content analysis to analyze the selected data and developed an analytical comparison through which he closely examined Napoleon"s character before and after the revolution. The findings of the study revealed that Napoleon was an opportunistic revolutionary who used the revolution to an evil end. Napoleon"s dramatic transformation from a noble revolutionary into a ruthless, corrupt ruler proved that Orwell"s novella can be read as an attack on false revolutionary leaders who become dehumanized despots, far worse than the dictators whom they aspired to replace with democratic leaders.

research paper on abuse of power

International Journal of Literature and Arts

Dlnya Mohammed

This paper studies the key roles that anthropomorphism plays in George Orwell's Animal Farm and Mark Twain's A Dog's Tale. In many ways, it is confusing when it comes to the difference between personification and anthropomorphism. To avoid this, the researcher makes a comparison between these two literary devices. Then the function of anthropomorphism is explained. Finally the purpose of anthropomorphism in the mentioned texts is made clear through textual analysis approach.

Prof.Dr.Abdul Ghafoor Awan

ABSTRACT-The main aim of this research paper is to analyze and explore Totalitarianism and its heinous impacts on any society. The other purpose of research paper is to take a deep look at Marxism with its history, principles and ideologies. The next target is exploring the terms utopia and dystopia and their creation. The next important aim of this research paper is to interlink all these terms; Totalitarianism, Marxism and Dystopia. One causes the other. The purpose of main study is the impacts of totalitarianism and Marxism towards the creation of dystopia. George Orwell’s selected fictions include Animal Farm (1945) and Nineteen Eighty-Four (1949). Both of these novels fulfill the above mentioned themes and for the first time all these themes will be connected and analyzed according to these two master pieces. Totalitarianism is a form of tyrant government in which a dictator rules the country according to his own wish and desire. History is full of such totalitarian leaders. The current research paper describes the history of the Totalitarianism and totalitarian dictators. The next resort is Marxism which is work of Karl Marx and he openly described themes, principles and ideologies of Marxism. Marxist study is embellished with capitalism in which a ruler or the upper class works to get control of the economy while exploiting the lower class. The dangerous impact of totalitarianism and capitalism is that it generates dystopian society and George Orwell’s novels are perfect choice for this research paper.

Mamadou Moustapha Sangharé

Ardeniz Özenç

George Orwell, the penname of Eric Arthur Blair, is one of the most prominent English writers of 20th century (Ash). He is best known for his fable Animal Farm, and his dystopian novel Nineteen Eighty-Four (Rossi and Rodden 8-10). Orwell is generally considered to be a “political writer” (Rossi and Rodden 1). However his political views conformed to neither communism nor capitalism, which were the major political ideologies that governed the world politics in the first half of the 20th century. He has a unique understanding of socialism that contradicts the Stalinist Communism of his age and capitalist ideology in general. His idea of socialism is based on a classless, egalitarian society in which the state has the responsibility to provide its citizens with equal rights and equal opportunities, so that every individual is capable of thinking for themselves. Especially, for the purpose of drawing attention to the conditions of the poor and oppressed, Orwell got down among the poorest people and produced a body of work dealing with poverty and social injustice, as well as other works dealing with the violation of basic human rights by totalitarian oppression. The aim of this paper is to analyse the development of Orwell’s understanding of socialism through analysing some of his works: The Road to Wigan Pier, Homage to Catalonia, Animal Farm and Nineteen Eighty-Four.

Michael Makovi

George Orwell is famous for his two final fictions, Animal Farm and Nineteen Eighty-Four. These two works are sometimes understood to defend capitalism against socialism. But as Orwell was a committed socialist, this could not have been his intention. Orwell's criticisms were directed not against socialism per se but against the Soviet Union and similarly totalitarian regimes. Instead, these fictions were intended as Public Choice-style investigations into which political systems furnished suitable incentive structures to prevent the abuse of power. This is demonstrated through a study of Orwell's non-fiction works, where his opinions and intentions are more explicit.

Majid Khorsand

Mubarak Muhammad

RELATED PAPERS

Goran O . Mustafa

Rıdvan Korkut

International Journal of Pluralism and Economics Education

SMART M O V E S J O U R N A L IJELLH

Susan McHugh

Iain Munro , Julie Labatut , john desmond

Rebin Najmalddin

International Journal of Science and Research

Amin Amirdabbaghian

SKIREC Publication- UGC Approved Journals

Harjot Singh

The Cambridge Companion to George Orwell

Morris Dickstein

Th. Horan (ed.), Critical Insights: Animal Farm, Ipswich Mass., Salem Press, 2018

Dario Altobelli

Ilha do Desterro A Journal of English Language, Literatures in English and Cultural Studies

Amin Amirdabbaghian , KRISHNAVANIE SHUNMUGAM

Andrew N. Rubin

Farhad Ahmad

Tom Ue, FRHistS

Joshua B . Gardiner

Talita Panikashvili

Anastacia Babenko

RELATED TOPICS

  •   We're Hiring!
  •   Help Center
  • Find new research papers in:
  • Health Sciences
  • Earth Sciences
  • Cognitive Science
  • Mathematics
  • Computer Science
  • Academia ©2024

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

Preview improvements coming to the PMC website in October 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List
  • Front Psychol

Examining the Relationship Between Leaders' Power Use, Followers' Motivational Outlooks, and Followers' Work Intentions

Taylor peyton.

1 School of Hospitality Administration, Boston University, Boston, MA, United States

2 Valencore Consulting, Cambridge, MA, United States

Drea Zigarmi

3 The Ken Blanchard Companies, Escondido, CA, United States

4 University of San Diego, San Diego, CA, United States

Susan N. Fowler

From the foundation of self-determination theory and existing literature on forms of power, we empirically explored relationships between followers' perceptions of their leader's use of various forms of power, followers' self-reported motivational outlooks, and followers' favorable work intentions. Using survey data collected from two studies of working professionals, we apply path analysis and hierarchical multiple regression to analyze variance among constructs of interest. We found that followers' perceptions of hard power use by their leaders (i.e., reward, coercive, and legitimate power) was often related to higher levels of sub-optimal motivation in followers (i.e., amotivation, external regulation, and introjected regulation). However, followers who perceived their leaders used soft power (i.e., expert, referent, and informational power) often experienced higher levels of optimal motivation (i.e., identified regulation and intrinsic motivation), but further investigation of soft power use is warranted. The quality of followers' motivational outlooks was also related to intentions to perform favorably for their organizations.

Introduction

This study merges two fields of investigation: forms of leadership power stemming from empirical research on the psychology of power over the last five decades, and motivational outlooks from research on self-determination theory (SDT) over the last 40 years. Researchers in both areas have called for greater in-depth exploration of the relationship between leadership and motivation in organizational settings (eg., Elias, 2008 ; Stone et al., 2009 ; Meyer et al., 2010 ; Randolph and Kemery, 2011 ; Anderson and Brion, 2014 ).

Much of the research on the psychology of power is “still largely removed from the complexities and confounds of behavior in organizational settings” (Anderson and Brion, 2014 , p. 85). While laboratory studies have certain strong advantages, future research will need to grapple with the dynamics of interpersonal and psychological interactions, and related implications for organizational life (Anderson and Brion, 2014 ).

Podsakoff and Schriesheim ( 1985 ) called for investigations of the independent contributions different power bases make to explain variance in criterion variables relevant to subordinate outcomes. Also, Elias ( 2008 ) identified a need for more research on specific criteria that facilitate leaders' decisions regarding the kind of power they should exercise. In response to these calls and other apparent gaps in the literature, studies began to appear. For example, Mossholder et al. ( 1998 ) found that subordinates' perceptions of procedural justice fully mediated the relationship between ratings of their supervisors' use of five forms of social power, and job satisfaction and organizational commitment. Ward ( 1998 ) also studied subordinates' perceptions of their managers, and found that for four of eight aspects of psychological work climate, managerial power bases interacted with subordinates' manifest needs (achievement, dominance, autonomy, and affiliation). Notably, managers' use of personal power (expert and referent) had the biggest impact on psychological climate, especially when personal power use also occurred with reward power use (Ward, 1998 ). Politis ( 2005 ) examined relationships between five forms of managerial power and credibility, with employee knowledge acquisition attributes. Politis ( 2005 ) uncovered a positive relationship between expert power and knowledge acquisition attributes including negotiation, control, and personal traits; also, the study found that greater use of coercive and referent power related to lower levels of knowledge sharing and knowledge acquisition. Additionally, Pierro et al. ( 2012 ) discovered how supervisors' and subordinates' need for cognitive closure related to the efficacy and application of various social power bases, specifically regarding: employee preference for soft or hard power, subordinates' performance, and other organizational outcomes. Pierro et al. ( 2013 ) found positive relationships between transformational/charismatic leadership and subordinates' inclinations to comply with soft power, which was also indicative of higher levels of affective organizational commitment.

While the above studies demonstrate some of the work on power bases as they relate to criterion variables in accordance with calls from Podsakoff and Schriesheim ( 1985 ) and others, very few studies examine the connection between motivation and forms of power use, and SDT has not yet been comprehensively applied in these investigations. Power is an undeniable aspect of leadership, and we agree with other authors (i.e., Aguinis et al., 1994 ; Randolph and Kemery, 2011 ) who maintain that there is not enough is known about the degree to which employee perceptions of their managers' use of various forms of power is correlated with various forms of employee motivation.

Scholars in the field of SDT have made similar calls for more in-depth research connecting leadership behavior to motivation in organizational settings (e.g., Deci and Ryan, 2002 ; Stone et al., 2009 ). Some SDT researchers have requested a closer examination of how leadership qualities and interpersonal styles of managers influence their followers' tendencies to align their personal goals with organizational goals (i.e., Gagné and Deci, 2005 ). Other SDT researchers call for the examination of how leader behaviors can foster increased levels of intrinsic motivation in followers (e.g., Jungert et al., 2013 ). Still other SDT authors ask for research on how leaders might optimize employee engagement in organizational settings (Dysvik and Kuvaas, 2010 ).

A preponderance of the SDT literature and its core principles has been applied in settings other than business (Deci and Ryan, 2002 ; Ryan and Deci, 2017 ). While some business leaders may have a cursory awareness of the fundamental concepts of SDT (such as the basic psychological needs of autonomy, relatedness, and competence), few understand how to successfully support employees' needs in the face of organizational pressures for performance and output (Stone et al., 2009 ). Much of the present organizational psychology depends upon the “carrot and the stick” strategy for motivation and command-and-control methods for performance and behavior (Stone et al., 2009 ; Fowler, 2014 ). Here we explore the impact of various kinds of leadership power on non-power holders in organizations. We are interested in gaining insight into how a non-power holder's motivational outlooks may relate to their leaders' use of power in the workplace.

Purpose and Organization of the Study

This study aims to answer calls from researchers in both fields of investigation by empirically examining the relationship between leaders' use of various forms of power, followers' motivational outlooks, and followers' work intentions in organizations. Specifically, we propose to answer Podsakoff and Schriesheim ( 1985 ) call “to assess the independent contribution of each of the power bases to the variance explained in subordinate criterion variables” (p. 406). This study, is also directed toward the third research avenue suggested by “to consider putting future research efforts into those who are powerless” (Strum and Antonakis, 2015 , p. 157). In response, we focus on the intrapersonal experiences of followers who operate under their leaders' power. We examine the degree to which the perceived use of six forms of leader power might explain variance in follower motivation (i.e., its sub-forms), and follower work intentions. We consider existing literature on power and SDT to hypothesize that leaders' use of different kinds and combinations of power is connected to various motivational outlooks and work intentions in the non-power holder.

Review Of The Literature On Leader Power, Employee Motivation, And Employee Work Intention

Leader power.

Power typically entails a condition in which some individuals have control over resources and some do not. The term power is most commonly defined as “the asymmetric control over valued resources” (Anderson and Brion, 2014 , p. 69). Power relationships are inherently social and exist only in relation to others; parties with low power rely on parties with high power to obtain rewards and avoid punishment (Vince, 2014 ).

For leaders to be effective they must be able to shape the behavior of others (Elias, 2008 ). Forms of leader power that can be used to shape others behavior are embedded in people's psyches (Vince, 2014 ) through the structural features of today's organizations (Pfeffer, 1992 ; Clegg et al., 2006 ; Vince, 2014 ). Before the turn of this century, much of the literature concerned with leader power has been sociological or philosophical in origin and character (Elias, 2008 ; Anderson and Brion, 2014 ) and has traditionally dealt with the “social structure of corporations” (Clegg et al., 2006 , p. 387). The structural perspective of power (Pfeffer, 1992 ) has given rise to the foundation of systems of authority and the formation of legitimate social power relationships (e.g., Pfeffer, 1992 , 2011 ; DuBrin, 2009 ; Haugaard and Clegg, 2012 ; Lunenberg, 2012 ; Vince, 2014 ).

Leaders in the workplace may reinforce their power through their own demeanor and behavior, but they are ordained with their power by the organizational context (e.g., the authority to control resources, followers assigned to work under them), which includes higher-ranking small groups or strategic decision makers (Anderson and Brion, 2014 ). The antecedents for the bases for power stem from the social structure, the cultural patterned behavior of groups, and other practices within organizations (Lukes, 1974 , 2005 ). Much of the early literature on power was concerned with the character, skills and personality of the designated leader, with the organization's structures, policies, procedures, and with forms of hierarchy that authorize a leader's power (Foucault, 1982 ).

The bases of power (French and Raven's, 1959 ; Raven, 1965 ), which will be described next, define forms of power observed from human and contextual factors, whereas the psychology of power is concerned with how people perceive and experience the power bases, either when they hold the power themselves, or when they are under the power of others. Since the beginning of the twenty-first century, there has been a rise in the number of empirical studies on the psychology of power, as researchers have recently begun to explore psychological perceptions of power to better understand how power use affects individuals in an organizational context (Anderson and Brion, 2014 ). There has been a shift away from studying power as it resides in structures, policies, and procedures, and a shift toward studying individual perceptions that are held by power/non-power holders when various uses of power occur. Recent work on individual perceptions includes, for example: studies concerned with why power facilitates self-interested behavior (DeCelles et al., 2012 ); how people who are primed with high- vs. low-power tend to adopt the visual perspective of others, adjust to other people's points of view, and feel empathy for others (Galinsky et al., 2006 ); and how power influences people's thinking while resolving moral dilemmas (Lammers and Stapel, 2009 ). Additionally, we agree with other researchers (e.g., Farmer and Aguinis, 2005 ; Dambe and Moorad, 2008 ) who have noted that most studies on power have predominantly focused on the power holder and not on either the mutuality of the power holder and the non-power holder, or on the perspective of the individual non-power holder.

Various Bases of Power

French and Raven's ( 1959 ) and Raven ( 1965 ) presented five conceptual forms of leader power which have been the basis of 50 years of research (Elias, 2008 ). These five forms of leader power—expert, referent, reward, legitimate, and coercive power—have remained relatively constant over time, even though there have been controversial issues, such as response bias possibilities, concept overlap, and single-item measurement (cf. Podsakoff and Schriesheim, 1985 ). To date, French and Raven's ( 1959 ) five forms of power are frequently used for the study of power in organizations.

Subsequently, Raven refined the five concepts of power from the earlier 1959 publication by adding a sixth major form of power, informational power, and further differentiated types of legitimate power into legitimate reciprocity, legitimate equity, and legitimate dependence (Raven, 1993 ). Additionally, coercive power was divided into personal coercive power and impersonal coercive power (Raven et al., 1998 ), and reward power was separated into personal reward power and impersonal reward power (Raven et al., 1998 ). These changes were made in an effort to overcome some of the concerns raised by Podsakoff and Schriesheim ( 1985 ) and Yukl and Tracy ( 1992 ), among others (Raven et al., 1998 ).

Alternatively, some researchers have classified varying forms of power into two clusters, i.e., soft and hard power, based on the amount of perceived freedom employees have in responding to the types of power used by their managers (e.g., Raven et al., 1998 ; Pierro et al., 2008 ; Randolph and Kemery, 2011 ). Expert power, informational power, and referent power are referred to by these and other authors as soft forms of power, while coercive, reward, and legitimate power have been classified as hard forms of power. Hard types of power require higher levels of non-power holder compliance and result in lower levels of autonomy.

Expert power depends on perceptions of the follower regarding the influencer's superior knowledge (Raven et al., 1998 ). The strength of this power turns upon the amount of expertise or knowledge the follower attributes to the influencer on a specific topic (Podsakoff and Schriesheim, 1985 ). Informational power refers to the influencer's perceived capacity to provide a rationale to the follower regarding why the follower should change his or her beliefs or behaviors (Raven et al., 1998 ). Referent power is dependent on the follower's perceived personal identification with the influencer (Raven et al., 1998 ). The basis of this power stems from the extent to which the follower's personal self-identity is made better through interaction with the influencer or the desire of the follower to be like, or associated with, the influencer (Podsakoff and Schriesheim, 1985 ).

Coercive power is defined as the perceived ability of the leader to penalize the targets if they do not adhere to requested outcomes (Raven et al., 1998 ). The potency of coercive power lies in the perceived extent of the punishment possible, and its use often correlates with increased negative affect between leader and follower (Podsakoff and Schriesheim, 1985 ). Reward power originates from a perceived possibility of monetary or non-monetary compensation (Raven et al., 1998 ). The intensity of reward power heightens with an increase in the rewards possible, and with its relative attractiveness to the receiver (Podsakoff and Schriesheim, 1985 ). Legitimate power originates from the subordinate's perceived understanding of the leader's right to influence (Raven et al., 1998 ). The potency of legitimate power arises from the internalized values the follower has concerning the authority or right of the leader to be the leader (Podsakoff and Schriesheim, 1985 ).

Correlates and Outcomes of Power

The nature of power is inherently a double-edged sword in which some people have prerogatives and others do not. This realization has given rise to research on self-interested behavior and the misuse of various forms of power, the subsequent examination of psychological explanations (e.g., Galinsky et al., 2006 ; Lammers and Stapel, 2009 ; DeCelles et al., 2012 ), and resultant possible solutions, such as empowerment (e.g., Conger and Kanungo, 1988 ; Randolph and Kemery, 2011 ).

Research studies in the last two decades reveal that the use of various forms of power correlates with various desirable and undesirable organizational and individual outcomes. For example, greater use of soft kinds of power (expert, referent, and informational power) are connected to higher levels of organizational citizenship behavior, empowerment, organizational commitment, and job satisfaction (e.g., Podsakoff and Schriesheim, 1985 ; Elias, 2008 ; Randolph and Kemery, 2011 ), whereas the use of hard forms of power (coercive, reward, and legitimate) are related to greater absenteeism, lower productivity, lower self-confidence levels, and burnout (Podsakoff and Schriesheim, 1985 ; Elias, 2008 ; Randolph and Kemery, 2011 ).

Employee Motivation and Self-Determination Theory

The concept of motivation in this study originates from the fundamental tenets of SDT, which have been researched and confirmed in the past five decades. This theory holds that individuals are volitional, able to initiate behaviors (Deci and Ryan, 1985 , 2002 ) and that individuals thrive when their psychological needs are satisfied (Deci and Ryan, 1985 , 2002 ). SDT purports that the individual cognitively process their experience which results in self-direction through flexible psychological structures that allow individuals to direct action toward the achievement of desired ends (Ryan and Deci, 2017 ).

Additionally, SDT researchers (e.g., Deci and Ryan, 1985 ; Ryan and Deci, 2000 ) maintain that an individual's actions are self-determined when they are chosen and supported by personally defined boundaries rather than being coerced, pressured, or induced through incentives. The term self-determination connotes a sense of self-management or self-regulation that, over time, brings with it goal direction, energy, persistence, and intention (Ryan and Deci, 2000 , 2017 ). In other words, individuals can understand why they behave the way they do. SDT purports that individuals can understand the causality of their actions, develop causality orientations (implicit and explicit, Deci and Ryan, 1985 ), and regulate their future behaviors to be congruent with such orientations.

SDT emphasizes that individuals' psychological needs for autonomy, relatedness, and competence must be fulfilled (Deci and Ryan, 1985 , 2002 ). Rather than focusing upon the lessening of physiological drives of sex, hunger, thirst, and pain avoidance, lasting human motivation originates from intrinsic needs for integration and growth (Deci and Ryan, 1985 ). Given that there is a spectrum of needs, frequent interactions with the environment allow for the fulfillment of basic psychological needs for thriving and flourishing, that encompasses more than physiological satiation (Deci and Ryan, 2002 ). The basic psychological needs of autonomy, relatedness, and competence, often designated as “psychological nutriments,” are as mandatory as physiological nourishment for human psychological development and well-being (Ryan and Deci, 2000 , p. 75). These basic psychological needs can give rise to various forms of motivational regulation and their associated motivational outlooks.

SDT defines two broad categories of motivational regulation: controlled regulation and autonomous regulation. Controlled regulation entails participation in an activity for instrumental reasons, rather than for reasons of pleasure or being interested in the activity for the sake of the activity itself (Gagné and Deci, 2005 ; Meyer et al., 2010 ). Autonomous regulation is designated as a person's participation in an activity for its own sake, because it is pleasurable or because it is of interest (Gagné and Deci, 2005 ; Meyer et al., 2010 ). Within controlled and autonomous regulation, SDT postulates various sub-categories or motivational outlooks: external, introjected, identified, and intrinsic. In this paper, we use the terms for motivation, such as motivational outlooks or forms regulation offered by Gagné et al. ( 2015 ).

An external motivational outlook is driven by desired rewards or punishment avoidance (i.e., controlled regulation). An introjected motivational outlook is connected to ego enhancement or to the avoidance of guilt or shame (i.e., controlled regulation). External and introjected motivational outlooks are classified as controlled regulation, in that they originate from instrumental outcomes or external conditions (Gagné and Deci, 2005 ).

An identified motivational outlook is a state in which the individual participates in activities to be congruent with valued personal goals (i.e., autonomous regulation). The identified motivational outlook usually stems from willful actions that adhere to stated values. If, after reflection, the individual believes he/she has chosen at will to engage in an activity because it is congruent with his/her fundamental needs and values, a sense of autonomy is obtained (Ryan and Deci, 2000 ; Gagné and Deci, 2005 ). And finally, an intrinsic motivational outlook is a state in which a personal sense of self is expressed by the individual when participating in an activity (i.e., autonomous regulation) (Gagné and Deci, 2005 ; Meyer et al., 2010 ). Identified or intrinsic motivational outlooks are categorized as autonomous regulation.

Apart from the motivational regulations, a state of amotivation, or disinterest, can occur when people lack the volition to act—or act passively—toward a specific outcome. Amotivation may exist because of forces beyond the individual's control. This feeling of helplessness may stem from uncontrollable or unpredictable environmental factors, or it could happen because the individual was overwhelmed by thoughts and feelings from within, such as anger, rage, resignation, or despair (Deci and Ryan, 1985 ). It is possible that employees may carry out their tasks mindlessly and without purpose or care, with little regard for their performance. External and introjected regulation are different from amotivation because, in the former, the motivation of the individual expresses a modicum of volition for some specific outcome.

We use the language “sub-optimal” and “optimal” to broadly refer to clusters of motivational outlooks or states, and the distinction is based on each state's support of sustainable, long-term human flourishing. Sub-optimal motivational outlooks include amotivation, external regulation, and introjected regulation; they are classified together as sub-optimal, in that an individual's energy toward a given task is approached from a lack of interest, or from a psychological place other than positive interest or value-congruent reasons. Thus, employee performance originating from any of the sub-optimal motivational outlooks, over the long haul, will either be characterized by a lack of effort or will likely not be sustainable. Alternatively, we classify identified and intrinsic motivational outlooks (i.e., autonomous regulation) as optimal, because they involve greater fulfillment of basic psychological needs, and therefore employee efforts stemming from optimal motivation are more likely to be sustainable.

Work Intentions

In keeping with the SDT, an individual's energy for “volitional, intentional behavior originates from underlying personal needs for autonomy, relatedness, and competence” (Deci, 1980 , p. 23). Here an intention is defined as a mental image of the behavior an individual plans to manifest (Bagozzi, 1992 ). Several studies have revealed that intentions are an important concept in the attitude-intention-behavior chain (e.g., Ajzen and Fishbein, 1980 ; Armitage and Connor, 2001 ). Studies testing social cognitive appraisal theory, for instance, have predicted work satisfaction from self-efficacy, positive affect, and work conditions (Duffy and Lent, 2009 ), have examined the relationship between control coping and employee withdrawal during organizational change (Fugate et al., 2011 ), have identified the relationship between appraisal/coping variables and stressful encounter outcomes (Folkman et al., 1986 ), and have uncovered relationships between consumers' behavioral intentions to use services in the future with consumer expectations, perceived quality, and satisfaction (Gotlieb et al., 1994 ).

In the fields of health and social psychology, various meta-analyses have demonstrated strong relationships between intentions and behavior (e.g., Cooke and Sheeran, 2004 ; Gollwitzer and Paschal, 2006 ; Webb and Sheeran, 2006 ). We chose to use the concept of work intentions as outcome variables because they are stronger predictors of employee behavior. Three meta-analyses conducted over the last 40 years have established that intentions are better predictors of employee behavior than outcome variables, such as organizational commitment and job satisfaction (e.g., Steel and Ovalle, 1984 ; Tett and Meyer, 1993 ; Podsakoff et al., 2007 ).

This research explores possible relationships between followers' perceptions of their leader's use of different kinds and combinations of power, various types of motivational outlooks in followers, and five work intentions held by followers. See Figure 1 for our conceptual model.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is fpsyg-09-02620-g0001.jpg

Overall conceptual model.

Very little empirical testing of the relationships between power and motivation exists in the literature, so the main impetus for our hypotheses is theoretical. Our underlying logic regarding followers' motivational outlooks assumes that the non-power holders' basic psychological need for autonomy, relatedness, and competence will be met or not met, facilitated are not facilitated, through the leader's use of various forms of power, hard or soft. The theoretical justification for these hypotheses lies in the followers' experience of the quality of: choice or autonomy given by the leader, relatedness cultivated by the leader, and competence experienced in relationship to the leader.

Specifically, we point to the research on human choice. Building on the work of SDT researchers, Patall et al. ( 2008 ) included 41 studies in a meta-analysis examining the effects of choice on intrinsic motivation. The authors concluded that intrinsic motivation was stronger when choice was given, and when rewards were not given. According to SDT, when a person's basic psychological needs (autonomy, relatedness, competence) are met, they thrive and behave in more self-determined (or optimally motivated) ways. Giving a person a choice relates to their experience of autonomy, one of the three basic psychological needs. Definitions of various forms of power (i.e., Podsakoff and Schriesheim, 1985 ; Raven et al., 1998 ; Pierro et al., 2008 ) suggest that coercive power, reward power, and legitimate (i.e., hard) power provide limited opportunity for the person under these types of power to exercise choice, compared to expert, referent, and informational (i.e., soft) power. If a leader's hard power limits follower choice much more than soft power, we anticipate followers' basic psychological needs and motivation will be affected accordingly.

Also, we found two empirical studies that examined different forms of power and motivation. First, Elangovan and Xie ( 1999 ), reported many positive relationships between subordinates' levels of internal motivation and forms of power being used by their supervisors, but effects were notably stronger for subordinates with low self-esteem or external locus of control. Second, Pierro et al. ( 2008 ) reported hard power compliance positively correlated with extrinsic motivation and negatively correlated with intrinsic motivation, whereas soft power compliance was positively correlated with intrinsic motivation. Pierro et al. ( 2008 ) and Elangovan and Xie ( 1999 ) both did not frame their studies primarily in SDT, so neither study included measures of motivation that captured the various subscales of motivation comprehensively defined by SDT.

Given the small number of studies exploring the relationship between leader power and subordinate motivation, more research is needed. Thus, applying conclusions from the research on human choice, from SDT, and from the above literature testing French and Raven's power bases in relationship to motivation, we propose:

  • Hypothesis 1a : Leaders' use of various kinds of hard power will positively correlate with followers' sub-optimal motivation (i.e., amotivation, external regulation, introjected regulation).
  • Hypothesis 1b : Leaders' use of various kinds of hard power will negatively correlate with, or not correlate with, followers' optimal motivation (i.e., identified regulation, intrinsic motivation).
  • Hypothesis 1c : Leaders' use of various kinds of soft power will negatively correlate with, or not correlate with, followers' sub-optimal motivation (i.e., amotivation, external regulation, introjected regulation).
  • Hypothesis 1d : Leaders' use of various kinds of soft power will positively correlate with followers' optimal motivation (i.e., identified regulation, intrinsic motivation).

Furthermore, leaders' use of power and followers' motivation may also relate to followers' work intentions. Zigarmi et al. ( 2016 ) examined employee locus of control, forms of motivational regulation, harmonious and obsessive passion, and desirable work intentions. While the direct connection between motivational regulation variables and work intentions in Zigarmi et al. ( 2016 ) was not estimated in their structural model, their correlation matrix showed: strong positive relationships between autonomous motivation and all five work intentions, small-to-medium negative relationships between amotivation and all work intentions, and small positive relationships between controlled motivation and three of five work intentions. Such relationships are in accordance with the assumptions of SDT, which purport that optimal motivation relates to human thriving.

Additionally, Zigarmi et al. ( 2015 ) found slight-to-moderate positive correlations between employees' perceptions of their leaders' use of expert and referent (i.e., soft) power and all favorable work intentions. In the same study, coercive and legitimate (i.e., hard) power were negatively and somewhat weakly correlated with five of ten possible work intentions, whereas reward power was somewhat positively correlated to all five work intentions. In their structural model that included positive and negative affect as mediators, Zigarmi et al. ( 2015 ) found that—except for expert power which had a negative, direct path to intent to perform—reward power and expert power each positively and directly related to two of five favorable work intentions. From that work, we observe variability in employees' intentions to perform well for their organization, relative to the kind of power their leaders use.

Considering the above, regarding the relationship between power, motivation, and work intentions, we propose:

  • Hypothesis 2a : Followers' sub-optimal motivation (i.e., amotivation, external regulation, introjected regulation) will negatively correlate with, or not correlate with, their work intentions.
  • Hypothesis 2b : Followers' optimal motivation (i.e., identified regulation, intrinsic motivation) will positively correlate with their work intentions.
  • Hypothesis 3a : Followers' motivational outlooks will partially mediate perceptions of leaders' use of various kinds of power and followers' work intentions.
  • Hypothesis 4a : Followers of leaders who use multiple kinds of hard power at high levels (as compared to leaders who use lower levels of all kinds of hard power) will report higher levels of amotivation, external regulation, and introjected regulation, lower levels of (or no difference in levels of) identified regulation and intrinsic motivation, and lower levels of (or no difference in levels of) work intentions.
  • Hypothesis 4b : Followers of leaders who use multiple kinds of soft power at high levels (as compared to leaders who use lower levels of all kinds of soft power) will report higher levels of identified regulation and intrinsic motivation, lower levels of (or no difference in levels of) amotivation, external regulation, and introjected regulation, and higher levels of work intentions.

Hypothesis 3a was written parsimoniously to address all substantive constructs of interest to us, as our approach to partial mediation analyses will be exploratory. Thus, any non-significant relationships we uncover from testing Hypotheses 1a − 1d and 2a − 2b will naturally affect the possibility to test partial mediation proposed by Hypothesis 3a.

In summary, we hypothesize that a leader's increased use of harder forms of power will be related to decreased quality of their followers' forms of motivation, and that less optimal motivation in employees will relate to lower levels of work intentions. Also, the more optimal forms of motivation in employees should correlate with higher levels of work intentions. While various studies we cited above provide some support for the connection between followers' work intentions relative to their motivation, and to their leaders' use of power, we have found no empirical study yet that has examined these factors together.

We conducted two studies to test our hypotheses. Study 1 involved a sample of respondents from a single organization, while Study 2 collected a larger sample of employees working across many organizations. Study 2 was conducted to determine if the findings from Study 1 could be replicated. Both studies were approved by the Research Ethics Committee of The Ken Blanchard Companies.

Participants for Study 1

Three-hundred seventy employees from a training and consulting organization in Southern California were invited to participate in Study 1. The sample for analysis included 229 employees, or a 62% response rate. Seventy percent of respondents were female, 78% were White/Caucasian, 22% were managers, and 60% reported being born in 1961 or later. Thirty percent had graduate degrees, 44% were college graduates, and 26% had some college education or less. Organizational tenure varied; 30% said they had been with their organization for 5 years or less, 21% reported a tenure of 6–10 years, 34% reported a tenure of 11–20 years, and 15% said they had worked for their organization for 21 years or more.

Procedures for Study 1

Participants from a single organization were invited through email to complete an online survey. Data for this study were gathered as part of a voluntary, anonymous, annual survey conducted by the company's human resources department.

In addition to demographic information, participants were asked to respond to subscales measuring their manager's use of power and the kinds of motivational outlooks they personally experience at work.

Managerial use of power was measured through the Interpersonal Power Inventory (IPI) from Raven et al. ( 1998 ). The IPI presents 11 subscales representing various power bases and is an extension of the original six power bases proposed by French and Raven's ( 1959 ) and subsequently Raven ( 1965 ). The IPI asks respondents to think of a time when they complied with their supervisor's request despite initially being reluctant to do so, then presents 33 items asking for their reason for compliance to be rated on a 7-point response scale (1 = definitely not a reason , 7 = definitely a reason ). To ensure parsimony and practicality in the interpretation of results, Study 1 combined the IPI's eleven subscales of power to measure the original six power bases: reward power, coercive power, legitimate power, expert power, referent power, and informational power. The reward and coercive power subscales each had six items, the legitimate power subscale included nine items, and the expert, referent, and informational power subscales were each made up of three items. Example items follow for each kind of power subscale used in this work: “My supervisor could help me receive special benefits” (reward power), “My supervisor may have been cold and distant if I did not do as requested” (coercive power), “I understood that my supervisor really needed my help on this” (legitimate power), “My supervisor probably knew more about the job than I did” (expert power), “I looked up to my supervisor and generally modeled my work accordingly” (referent power), and “My supervisor gave me good reasons for changing how I did the job” (informational power). Alpha coefficients for the power subscales in Study 1 ranged from 0.87 to 0.96.

Employee workplace motivation was measured using the Multidimensional Work Motivation Scale (MWMS). This 19-item work scale has been validated in seven languages (see Gagné et al., 2015 ). Participants were asked to respond to the item “why do you or would you put efforts into your current job” and were given a 7-point rating scale (1 = not at all , 7 = completely-entirely ) to indicate the degree to which each survey item represented their reasons for expending the effort to become involved with their job. The MWMS includes six subscales for workplace motivation: amotivation, external-social, external-material, introjected, identified, and intrinsic. For this study, researchers combined the external-social and external-material subscales to create a total score for external regulation, such that we included five dimensions for motivation. Three, six, four, three, and three items, respectively, made up our five forms of motivational outlooks: amotivation, external regulation, introjected regulation, identified regulation, and intrinsic motivation subscales. An example item for motivation is “Because the work I do is interesting” (intrinsic motivation).

For Study 1, alpha coefficients for amotivation and introjected regulation were below 0.70, and respective results should, therefore, be interpreted with caution. Gagné et al. ( 2015 ) tested the psychometric properties of all MWMS subscales in various cultural contexts and cited borderline or inadequate reliability (≤0.70) for the introjected regulation subscale in three of seven samples. However, Gagné et al. cited adequate reliability (>0.70) for the amotivation subscale in all samples, so issues with the measurement of amotivation were surprising. The reliabilities of all other motivation subscales for Study 1 were adequate, ranging from 0.80 to 0.90.

Also, in Study 1 there was a strong positive skew of the amotivation subscale, which was initially problematic for analysis; for the three items for this subscale, 81–89% of respondents indicated that they were “not at all” experiencing amotivation at work. To work with the data, we dichotomized the amotivation items so that those who were experiencing no amotivation at all were coded in a separate category from respondents reporting any level of amotivation at work, and subscale average total scores were calculated from these variables prior to analysis. This treatment of the data was beneficial because it lessened the strength of the positive skew of the amotivation variable to be used in subsequent regression analyses.

Employee work intentions data were collected from the short form of the Work Intentions Inventory (WII) (Nimon and Zigarmi, 2015 ), and this inventory was included within the surveys used for Studies 1 and 2. Over the past 4 years, two versions of the work intentions inventory have been developed. The initial WII long form (Zigarmi et al., 2012 ) contained 25 items while the WII short form included 15 items. In each WII version, 5 types of work intentions are represented (Nimon and Zigarmi, 2015 ): intent to use discretionary effort, intent to perform at a higher than average level, intent to endorse, intent to stay with the organization, and intent to use organizational citizenship behavior. The WII has demonstrated construct and content validity for the five work intentions subscales and has repeatedly displayed appropriate internal consistency and factorial structure (Nimon and Zigarmi, 2015 ).

The WII offers a six-point Likert-type response scale format to capture the respondent's extent of intention experienced, ranging from 1 = no extent to 6 = the fullest extent . For the WII short form used in this study, each of the five work intentions subscales were measured with three items. Example items include: “I intend to take home work when I know it will make me more effective the next day” (intent to use discretionary effort), “I intend to exert the energy it takes to do my job well” (intent to perform), “I intend to talk positively about this organization to my friends and family” (intent to endorse), “I intend to continue to work here because I believe it is the best decision for me” (intent to stay), and “I intend to watch out for the welfare of others at work” (intent to use OCB). In this work, reliabilities for the five intentions subscales ranged from 0.77 to 0.93 in Study 1.

Demographics

Age and gender were included in this study because these employee characteristics may relate to motivational outlooks at work (cf. Gagné et al., 2015 ). Both demographics questions used in Study 1 were part of a pre-existing pool of items regularly launched by the participating organization, so researchers were unable to alter their format. Age in Study 1 was measured using a single survey item that asked for respondents to indicate the year they were born by selecting among ranges of years. Some categories of age had too few respondents for analyses to be stable, so empirical criteria were used to dichotomize the age variable (born in 1960 or earlier vs. born in 1961 or later) in preparation for analysis. Gender was measured by asking respondents if they were male or female.

Participants for Study 2

An invitation to participate in the study was sent electronically to a listserv of ~40,000 employees across the United States. The sample of participants included 1,103 employees of various organizations, or a 3% response rate. Females made up 58% of the respondents. Data were not available on participants' ethnic or educational backgrounds, but 67% percent were from organizations operating within the United States. Thirty-three percent of respondents were non-managers, with an average age of 49. Forty-one percent had been with their current organization for more than 10 years, 55% had been in their current position for 4 years or less, and 70% had been reporting to their current supervisor for 4 years or less. Thirty-three percent worked for organizations with 500 or fewer employees, and 20% were from organizations with more than 20,000 employees.

Procedures for Study 2

Access to the listserv was granted by an international training and management company that works with organizations from various industries. Respondents were granted access to the company's white papers as an incentive for their participation.

For the second study, we used the same measures for power, motivation, and work intentions as we did in the first study (i.e., the IPI, MWMS, and WII). In Study 2, alpha reliabilities for the power subscales ranged from 0.84 to 0.93, and alpha reliabilities for the motivation subscales ranged from 0.70 to 0.89—notable improvements from reliability issues observed from these subscales in Study 1. Specifically, in Study 2, the reliability of amotivation and introjected regulation was acceptable at 0.85 and 0.70, respectively. Also, in Study 2 a lower percentage of respondents reported being “not at all” amotivated at work (i.e., 67–72% for the three amotivation items compared to 81–89% in Study 1), so for Study 2 the amotivation subscale was not dichotomized and was instead calculated from the amotivation items in their original, continuous format. In Study 2, alpha reliabilities for the work intentions subscales ranged from 0.73 to 0.95.

To measure respondent demographics, age and gender were again included in Study 2. In this launch, we had the option to redesign the age variable to better capture variability in respondents; thus, instead of the categorical, generationally-based scale for age used in Study 1, in Study 2 respondents were asked to type their age in years. No additional data manipulation was conducted on our age variable in the second study prior to analysis because it was analyzed as a continuous variable in our regression analyses in Study 2.

Study 2 used a larger sample spanning organizations across North America, and provided improved reliability coefficients for amotivation and introjected regulation compared to Study 1.

Evaluating Common Method Bias

We aimed to examine intrapersonal psychological phenomena for this work, so we collected self-report data. Self-report data is most appropriate for learning about respondents' inner experiences and perceptions, which were of focal interest here. Specifically, Chan ( 2009 ) highlighted the valuable insight self-report data may provide for researchers aiming to investigate potential affective, cognitive, and motivational processes implicated with individuals' response patterns. Some researchers have expressed concern about the possibility of relationship inflation among substantive constructs in self-report studies incorporating one source of data only (Spector, 2006 ), and the potential for such constructs to vary due to significant common method bias effects (Podsakoff and Organ, 1986 ; Podsakoff et al., 2003 ). We implemented the following approaches to minimize the likelihood of having problems with common method bias. First, we used measures featuring different kinds of response scales, which helps prevent participants from being overly consistent or automatic in their answers. Specifically, participants were asked to rate on a 7-point scale their perceived reasons underlying their manager's behavior, and then they were asked to reflect on their job experiences using a 6-point and 7-point scale, each involving different response anchors. Second, in separate sections of the survey, we varied the instructions and referent for phenomena being rated (i.e., participants were asked to shift from rating their manager's behavior prior to rating their own motivations and intentions on the job).

To additionally probe for the negative effects of common method bias, we applied Harman's single factor procedure (Podsakoff and Organ, 1986 ; Podsakoff et al., 2003 ). While this technique for evaluating issues with common method variance may be limited, if only one factor accounts for the majority of variance in the data, that could indicate problems attributable to common method bias (Podsakoff et al., 2003 ). In both studies, we ran Harman's procedure using exploratory factor analysis. Four and three factors were evident in Study 1 and Study 2, respectively, whereby the first factor accounted for 21.7% (Study 1) and 24.1% (Study 2) of the variance. We also, in both studies, used the marker variable approach suggested by Williams et al. ( 2010 ), which involves testing five nested models. In both studies, we found: (1) our data fit the Method-U model better than the Method-C model (i.e., our marker was differentially associated with our variables of interest), and (2) our data did not fit significantly better for the Method-R model than the Method-U model, which indicated that common method bias was not a problem. Harman's procedure and the Williams et al. ( 2010 ) both provided evidence that issues with common method bias were not evident in either study.

Path Analysis Approach

We used Mplus 7.2 and maximum likelihood estimation (MLR) to run competing multivariate models to test our conceptual model involving leader power use, followers' motivational outlooks, and followers' work intentions (i.e., Hypotheses 1a−1d, 2a−2b). Initially we attempted to conduct the full analysis as structural equation models by using latent variables for all substantive constructs, but for the smaller dataset in Study 1, we had too many parameters of interest to estimate, relative to our available statistical power. We therefore modeled both samples using path analysis, which only included two latent variables, one for soft power and one for hard power, and all other key variables were observed scale scores.

At the measurement model level, hard power as a latent variable was calculated from three mean scale scores: reward power, coercive power, and legitimate power. Our latent variable for soft power was calculated from the following three mean scale scores: expert, referent, informational power. Measurement model fit for the power variables was not adequate initially, but model modification indices showed that coercive power also should cross-load onto soft power. Upon making this modification, model fit improved (Δχ 2 [1] = 40.28, p < 0.001) and was well-fitting to the data (χ 2 [7] = 57.527, CFI = 0.902, SRMR = 0.060, RMSEA = 0.182). The cross-loading of coercive power onto soft power was −0.544 in Study 1.

Because we anticipated certain kinds of motivational outlooks would be highly related, we allowed them to correlate: i.e., external and introjected regulation were correlated, and so were identified and intrinsic motivation. In accordance with theory and previous research conducted on work intentions, we also specified correlations among the five work intentions. All path models controlled for respondent age and gender.

Overall model fit was evaluated using the following indices: the comparative fit index (CFI), the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), and the standardized root mean square residual (SRMR). Specifically, we retained models demonstrating CFI values >0.90, RMSEA values < 0.06, and SRMR values < 0.08 (Hu and Bentler, 1999 ; Hooper et al., 2008 ). Chi-square difference testing compared the fit of nested models for full and partial mediation. In each study, path analysis began by running a full mediation model in accordance with our overall conceptual model (Model 1), and that was followed by running variations of partial mediation models, whereby: starting with the full mediation model, five direct paths were added from hard power to each of the work intentions (Model 2); then starting again with the full mediation model, five direct paths were added from soft power to every work intention (Model 3); then all significant direct paths from Model 2 and Model 3 were noted so they could be added collectively to the full mediation model (Model 4); and then Model 4 was examined for non-significant direct paths so they could be removed for subsequent partial mediation model testing (Models 5–6).

Study 1 Path Analysis Results

Table ​ Table1 1 provides Study 1 variables' means, standard deviations, correlations, and alpha reliabilities. In Study 1, Models 1–3 were run as described above. Then, in Model 4, a partial mediation model was run by adding 6 direct paths: hard power to intent to perform, soft power to intent to perform, hard power to intent to endorse, soft power to intent to endorse, hard power to intent to use OCBs, and soft power to intent to use OCBs. Compared to Model 1, Models 2–4 fit the data better (Model 2: Δχ 2 [5] = 11.90, p < 0.05; Model 3: Δχ 2 [5] = 21.58, p < 0.001; Model 4: Δχ 2 [6] = 21.72, p < 0.01). Model 5 removed the non-significant direct paths found in Model 4, and only included 2 direct paths: soft power to intent to endorse, and soft power to intent to use OCBs. Model 5 was compared to Model 4 and did not fit the data better (Δχ 2 [4] = 8.72, p > 0.05). Results from chi-square significance testing to compare nested models is included in Table 2 . Model 4, shown in Figure 2 , fit the data best.

Study 1—scale score means, standard deviations, reliabilities, and correlations.

Cronbach's alpha estimates are in parentheses on the diagonal. Pairwise deletion, ns = 216–228. Gender coded as 1 = male, 2 = female. Age coded as 1 = born 1961 or later, 2 = born 1960 or earlier .

Chi-square significance testing for comparison of structural equation model fit.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is fpsyg-09-02620-g0002.jpg

Model 4, Final Model, Study 1 ( n = 215).

All standardized path coefficients for Model 4 are shown in Figure 2 , and respective endogenous variables' r 2 values were as follows: 0.25 for amotivation, 0.23 for external regulation, 0.072 for introjected regulation, 0.11 for identified regulation, 0.18 for intrinsic motivation, 0.23 for intent to use discretionary effort, 0.42 for intent to perform, 0.39 for intent to endorse, 0.35 for intent to stay, 0.37 for intent to use OCBs. Of the above listed r 2 values, only introjected regulation was not significant ( p > 0.05). Table 3 reports significant indirect effects.

Summary of significant, specific indirect effects for Model 4 in Study 1.

Only significant indirect effects are shown, p < 0.05 .

Study 2 Path Analysis Results

Variables' means, standard deviations, correlations, and alpha reliabilities for Study 2 are shown in Table 4 . Similar to Study 1, in Study 2 power variables' measurement model fit to the data was improved by allowing coercive power to load onto soft power (Δχ 2 [1] = 263.72, p < 0.001), resulting in a well-fitting measurement model for power (χ 2 [7] = 42.442, CFI = 0.981, SRMR = 0.025, RMSEA = 0.068). In Study 2, coercive power loaded onto soft power at −0.662. We ran structural Models 1–3 in the same way as previously presented in Study 1.

Study 2—scale score means, standard deviations, reliabilities, and correlations.

Cronbach's alpha estimates are in parentheses on the diagonal. Pairwise deletion, ns = 1,095–1,103. Gender coded as 1 = male, 2 = female. Age was a continuous variable with lower values representing younger ages .

Model 4 was a partial mediation model with the following 8 direct paths added: hard power to intent to use discretionary effort, soft power to intent to use discretionary effort, soft power to intent to perform, hard power to intent to endorse, soft power to intent to endorse, hard power to intent to stay, soft power to intent to stay, soft power to intent to use OCBs. Relative to Model 1, Models 2–4 fit the data better (Model 2: Δχ 2 [5] = 12.05, p < 0.05; Model 3: Δχ 2 [5] = 37.91, p < 0.001; Model 4: Δχ 2 [8] = 41.89, p < 0.001). Removing the non-significant direct paths from Model 4, Model 5 included 4 direct paths: soft power to intent to perform, soft power to intent to endorse, soft power to intent to stay, soft power to intent to use OCBs. Model 5 did not fit the data significantly better than Model 4 (Δχ 2 [4] = 8.78, p > 0.05). Applying the same logic, Model 6 included only 2 direct paths: soft power to intent to endorse, soft power to intent to stay. Model 6 did not show better fit to the data than Model 4 (Δχ 2 [6] = 11.19, p > 0.05). See Table 5 for model comparison results from chi-square significance testing, and Figure 3 for Model 4, which was best-fitting to the data.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is fpsyg-09-02620-g0003.jpg

Model 4, Final Model, Study 2 ( n = 1,039).

For Model 4, standardized path coefficients are presented in Figure 3 . For that final model, endogenous variables' r 2 values were: 0.18 for amotivation, 0.33 for external regulation, 0.13 for introjected regulation, 0.20 for identified regulation, 0.21 for intrinsic motivation, 0.20 for intent to use discretionary effort, 0.35 for intent Ω to perform, 0.32 for intent to endorse, 0.30 for intent to stay, 0.27 for intent to use OCBs. Of the above listed r 2 values, all were significant ( p < 0.001). Significant indirect effects are shown in Table 6 .

Summary of significant, specific indirect effects for Model 4 in Study 2.

Studies 1 And 2: Interpretation Of Final Models

Hypothesis 1a was mostly supported by both studies' final models, leaders' use of hard power was strongly and positively correlated with followers' amotivation and external regulation (sub-optimal motivational outlooks). For the path from hard power to amotivation, in Study 1 β = 0.42, p < 0.05, and in Study 2, β = 0.39, p < 0.05. Similarly, hard power was related to external regulation: Study 1 β = 0.54, p < 0.05, and in Study 2, β = 0.70, p < 0.05. A significant path between hard power and introjected regulation was only found in Study 2, however, possibly due to having more statistical power in that sample (β = 0.25, p < 0.05). Followers who perceived greater hard power use by their leaders were more likely to hold higher levels of sub-motivational outlooks at work.

Hypothesis lb was supported by the final model of Study 1, which found no significant relationships between leaders' use of hard power and identified or intrinsic motivation. In Study 2, this hypothesis was supported in that there were negative relationships between leaders' use of hard power and identified (β = −0.15, p < 0.05) and intrinsic (β = −0.20, p < 0.05) motivation. Thus, followers with leaders exerting higher levels of hard power use were somewhat more likely to report lower levels of optimal motivation.

In both studies, Hypothesis 1c was supported. Leaders' use of soft power was significantly and negatively related to followers' amotivation (β = −0.59, p < 0.05 in Study 1, and β = −0.53, p < 0.05 in Study 2) and to followers' external regulation (β = −0.20, p < 0.05 in Study 1, and β = −0.36, p < 0.05 in Study 2). In both studies, soft power was not significantly related to followers' introjected regulation. Followers who viewed higher amounts of soft power use by their leaders were more likely to report lower levels of amotivation and lower levels of external regulation.

Hypothesis 1d was supported by both studies, as soft power use by leaders positively correlated with followers' optimal motivational outlooks. Leaders' use of soft power was significantly related to followers' identified regulation (β = 0.29, p < 0.05 in Study 1, and β = 0.49, p < 0.05 in Study 2) and to followers' intrinsic motivation (β = 0.34, p < 0.05 in Study 1, and β = 0.53, p < 0.05 in Study 2). Followers who perceived higher soft power use from their leaders were more likely to report higher levels of optimal motivation.

Evidence supporting Hypothesis 2a was found in both studies, as followers' sub-optimal motivational outlooks (i.e., amotivation, external regulation, introjected regulation) either negatively correlated with, or did not correlate with, their work intentions. Four of five paths from amotivation to work intentions were negative and significant (i.e., for intent to perform β = −0.25, p < 0.05 in Study 1, and β = −0.21 p < 0.05 in Study 2; for intent to endorse β = −0.28, p < 0.05 in Study 1, and β = −0.17 p < 0.05 in Study 2; for intent to stay β = −0.32, p < 0.05 in Study 1, and β = −0.13, p < 0.05 in Study 2; for intent to use OCBs β = −0.30, p < 0.05 in Study 1, and β = −0.22, p < 0.05 in Study 2). Therefore, followers experiencing greater levels of amotivation were more likely to score lower on four of the five work intentions. Paths between amotivation and intent to use discretionary effort were not significant in either study. External regulation and introjected regulation were not significantly related to any of the work intentions in either study.

Hypothesis 2b was supported by both studies in eight of ten possible paths: for the most part, followers' optimal motivational outlooks (i.e., identified, intrinsic motivation) positively and significantly correlated with their work intentions. Identified regulation was positively related to the all five work intentions in Study 1 (βs ranged from 0.18 to 0.53, p < 0.05) and in Study 2 (βs ranged from 0.09 to 0.45, p < 0.05). Positive and significant paths were also found from intrinsic motivation to intent to use discretionary effort (β = 0.27, p < 0.05 in Study 1, and β = 0.19, p < 0.05 in Study 2), to intent to endorse (β = 0.15, p < 0.05 in Study 1, and β = 0.19 p < 0.05 in Study 2) and to intent to stay (β = 0.30, p < 0.05 in Study 1, and β = 0.24 p < 0.05 in Study 2). Taken together, except for intent to perform and intent to use OCBs, followers with higher levels of optimal motivation were more likely to report favorable levels of work intentions.

In both studies, partial mediation models fit the data better than complete mediation models, thereby somewhat supporting Hypothesis 3a . Finally, regarding direct paths from power to work intentions, followers who perceived their leaders used higher levels of soft power were more likely to intend to endorse their organizations (β = 0.25, p < 0.05 in Study 1, and β = 0.20, p < 0.05 in Study 2) and to intend to use OCBs (β = 0.21, p < 0.05 in Study 1, and β = 0.07, p < 0.05 in Study 2). In Study 2 only, followers working under leaders who used soft power reported increased intentions to stay with their organization (β = 0.27, p < 0.05).

Regression Approach

As a follow-up analysis to supplement our path modeling, we used hierarchical multiple regression analysis in SPSS version 22, which tested Hypotheses 4a and 4b to explain variance in the five motivational outlooks and the five work intentions. Specifically, we were interested in evaluating the potential effects of followers' experiences of their leaders' use of multiple kinds of hard or soft power in combination with one another.

In preparation for this analysis, in each study's dataset, we transformed power use so it could be meaningfully aggregated to indicate followers' perceptions of the degree of each kind of power was being used by their leader. As the subscale for the power measures ranged from 1 (to no extent) to 6 (to the fullest extent), we examined the labels of the response scales as well as the distributions of responses in each study, to determine that values of 4.5 or higher would allow for the meaningful dichotomization of followers' perceptions of their managers. That is, for every power variable, we re-coded followers with ratings of 4.49 and lower (i.e., the lower half of the “to a great extent” anchor and including all responses through “to no extent”) as 1s, and we re-coded followers with power ratings of 4.5 and higher (i.e., the upper half of the “to a great extent” anchor and including responses indicating “to a very great extent” and “to the fullest extent”) as 2s. Then, for each respondent, we summed the recoded scores on all hard power variables (coercive, reward, legitimate), to create a single variable that indicated the degree of hard power used by their leader, and aggregating across all types of hard power. Thus, scores for this new sum combination variable for hard power ranged from 3 to 6, with 3 indicating that a follower's leader uses low levels of all kinds of hard power, and 6 indicating that a follower's leader uses high levels of all kinds of hard power. The same transformation was done for soft power, prior to regression analysis.

In the regression analyses, respondent age and gender were entered into Step 1, and the two sum combination variables for hard and soft power were then also entered into Step 2. The mean imputation procedure within SPSS was conducted for cases with missing data.

Studies 1 And 2: Regression Results And Interpretation

Follow-up regression results are shown in Table 7 for Study 1, and in Table 8 for Study 2. Hypothesis 4a was supported in both studies, as followers of leaders who used multiple kinds of hard power at high levels demonstrated higher levels of amotivation (β = 0.16, p < 0.05 in Study 1, and β = 0.18, p < 0.05 in Study 2), external regulation (β = 0.41, p < 0.05 in Study 1, and β = 0.44, p < 0.05 in Study 2), and introjected regulation (β = 0.15, p < 0.05 in Study 1, and β = 0.23, p < 0.05 in Study 2). When leaders engaged in using many kinds of hard power, sub-optimal motivation levels in followers were more pronounced. Additionally, Hypothesis 4a was also confirmed in both studies as no significant relationships were found between followers' perceptions of their leaders' use of multiple kinds of hard power and follower's optimal motivational outlooks, or followers' work intentions.

Study 1—combinations of power regression models, controlling for age and gender ( n = 229).

β value shows asterisk if p < 0.05. Higher values for age represent older respondents .

Study 2—combinations of power regression models, controlling for age and gender ( n = 1,103).

In accordance with Hypothesis 4b , followers of leaders who used many kinds of soft power at high levels reported higher levels of identified (β = 0.15, p < 0.05 in Study 1, and β = 0.26, p < 0.05 in Study 2), and intrinsic motivation (β = 0.19, p < 0.05 in Study 1, and β = 0.31, p < 0.05 in Study 2). Followers with leaders engaging in multiple types of soft power also demonstrated significantly lower amounts of amotivation (β = −0.31, p < 0.05 in Study 1, and β = −0.22, p < 0.05 in Study 2), no significant change in external regulation (in both studies), and in Study 2 only, somewhat significantly higher levels of introjected regulation (β = 0.12, p < 0.05; note, however, the Study 1 β looked similar despite not being significant, which may be due to statistical power). Except for intent to use discretionary effort in Study 1, followers who perceived their leaders used many kinds of soft power were more likely to report favorable levels of work intentions (βs ranged from 0.19 to 0.31, p < 0.05 in Study 1, and βs ranged from 0.18 to 0.29, p < 0.05 in Study 2). See Table 9 for a summary of all hypotheses and results for both studies.

Summary of hypotheses and results for studies 1 and 2.

This work calls attention to the types of power that, when used by leaders, are more likely to relate to optimal or sub-optimal motivational outlooks in followers, and varying levels of followers' work intentions. Our findings highlight the benefits of when leaders use soft power with their followers, and the psychological costs to followers when leaders operate from hard power. This study demonstrates SDT is a relevant lens for understanding the psychology of followers working under power, and suggests the value of SDT in organizations.

Importantly, followers who experience their leaders using hard power will be much more likely to have sub-optimal motivational outlooks, particularly amotivation and external regulation. Furthermore, a highly notable finding was that leaders' use of multiple kinds of hard power together will correlate with all types of sub-optimal motivational outlooks in followers, and the strength of this relationship for external regulation is important for practice. This finding aligns with SDT literature and previous research that has provided evidence for negative outcomes associated with the use of hard power in organizations (Podsakoff and Schriesheim, 1985 ; Elias, 2008 ; Randolph and Kemery, 2011 ).

In some cases, followers who perceive their leaders use hard power may also suffer from a slight decline in optimal motivation. Results differed between our studies in the relationship between leaders' hard power use and followers' optimal motivation (i.e., Study 1 found no significant relationships, whereas Study 2 uncovered small negative, significant relationships). We wonder if the findings of Study 2 have more generalizability than Study 1, given that Study 2 was a large sample comprising followers from various organizations, whereas Study 1 was a small sample of followers from a single organization. The observed differences could be due to statistical power, or it is possible that employees from the same organization may collectively experience other factors that could mitigate the psychological sting of hard power. In general, we wonder if the latter may be the case when followers under their leaders' hard power have learned that their leaders still value them overall, as indicated by other cultural norms (e.g., one-on-one lunch outings, expressing the deeper meaning of the work, caring conversations, being granted extra flexibility in their work schedules). Or, perhaps in organizational cultures that normalize autocratic rule from leaders (e.g., military settings), employees may expect to often experience hard power and therefore be less personally affected by it.

Additionally, followers who perceive soft power use from their leaders will be notably more likely to experience optimal motivational outlooks. Followers working with leaders who use higher amounts of soft power may benefit from feeling lower levels of sub-optimal motivation, specifically for amotivation and external regulation. Additionally, when leaders use many kinds of soft power at once, followers' motivational outlooks may benefit by being more optimal, and less characterized by amotivation. The potential compounding psychological effect felt by followers who had leaders using multiple kinds of power was evident in this study, and is in alignment with SDT. Mainly, leaders who exercise many kinds of hard power at once (or many kinds of soft power at once), may be more strongly depleting or enriching followers' basic psychological needs of autonomy, relatedness, and competence than leaders who only use one kind of power.

The quality of followers' motivational outlooks also appears to be connected to followers' intentions to engage in positive work outcomes for their organizations. Amotivation in followers tends to decrease followers' intentions to perform, endorse their organizations, stay with their organizations, and use OCBs, although followers' intentions for discretionary effort may remain unaffected by experiences of amotivation. The other two kinds of followers' sub-optimal motivational outlooks, external regulation and introjected regulation, are not related to followers' work intentions. Conversely, optimal motivational outlooks in followers are very often related to followers' favorable work intentions. These findings strongly support assumptions of SDT, as it is expected that followers who are more optimally motivated will have their basic psychological needs met, and therefore they will have greater capacity to function from a place of strength and resilience at work.

Furthermore, leaders' use of multiple kinds of soft power will relate to the increased likelihood that their followers will intend to work positively for their organization. This relationship was observed in all work intentions variables across both studies, except for in Study 1 where combinations of soft power use did not correlate with followers' intent to use discretionary effort. This may suggest that employees in Study 1, our organizational sample, collectively have other reasons besides soft power to exert strong effort in their work (e.g., recognition, appreciation, teamwork mentality). Also, the final model in Study 2 had a positive and significant direct path between leaders' use of soft power and followers' intentions to stay with their organization, whereas Study 1 did not. Again, the differences in findings could be attributable to statistical power, or something else may be explaining Study 1 followers' variability in their intentions to stay. Qualitatively, we know that employees in the organization used in Study 1 often have high tenure, so it could be that followers' intention to stay with that organization is less related to leader soft power, and perhaps more related to other benefits the company offers.

Overall, the magnitude of the relationships between followers' optimal motivational outlooks and work intentions is relevant for practice; these findings suggest that leading in a manner that encourages others to form and maintain identified and intrinsic motivational outlooks is not only a practice that sounds ideal in principle, but indeed it is a practice that could add great value to workplace outcomes.

Taken together, for leaders who wish to promote sustainable and healthy kinds of motivation in their followers, the above suggests that using soft power is a superior approach to using hard power. Considering the basic tenants SDT, our findings may suggest that leaders' use of hard power will likely disrupt followers' psychological flourishing in the areas of autonomy, relatedness, and competence—and thereby will impair followers' motivation, and their intentions to perform favorably on the job. Thus, we raise the question: are organizational leaders who rule from hard power ultimately undermining themselves?

Limitations and Recommendations for Future Research

Our findings regarding the introjected motivational outlook were generally inconclusive, so we recommend additional research in that area. It should be noted that Gagné ( 2016 ), in a paper presented at the SDT Conference in June of 2016, said that different sub-forms of introjection may be at work in the larger concept of introjection. Gagné offered that the definition of introjection may be incomplete, such that it may include shame and guilt as subconstructs. The nascent state of introjection as an academic concept may be affecting its measurement in our studies and, therefore, may be shaping our results accordingly. It may be worthwhile to investigate whether introjected motivational outlooks may be connected to other context-specific factors besides managerial use of power, or whether perhaps introjected motivational outlooks may instead be more strongly connected to individual personality differences or social axioms held by employees.

Effect decomposition analyses for the path models specifying hard and soft power uncovered total indirect effects between hard power and work intentions, through all motivational outlook variables. Specifically, regarding indirect effects flowing through motivational outlooks, in both studies, employees with managers using higher amounts of hard power were somewhat less likely to report favorable levels of work intentions. The opposite, and slightly stronger, indirect effect was found for employees with managers exercising higher levels of soft power; these employees reported greater intentions to work favorably. Therefore, managerial use of power was related to more than employees' motivational outlooks; productive levels of work intentions were also related to power use. These findings are in keeping with Zigarmi et al. ( 2016 ), whose research revealed correlations between amotivation and work intentions ( r s ranged from −0.14 to −0.31), controlled regulation and work intentions ( r s ranged from not significant to 0.13), and autonomous regulation and work intentions ( r s ranged from 0.42 to 0.56). Future research could investigate causal relationships between perceived leader power, employee motivation, and intentions, or other aspects of organizational life that may be influenced by managerial use of certain kinds of power.

Low reliabilities of the amotivation and introjected regulation subscales in Study 1 indicate that conclusions drawn from Study 1 regarding those variables should be made with caution. Also, although demographic effects on motivation differed somewhat between studies, we are optimistic that comparing findings across studies and controlling for demographic differences strengthened our conclusions.

Both studies were convenience samples, so findings may not generalize to broader populations. Additionally, because data were cross-sectional, this study does not provide evidence for the directionality of relationships observed. Thus, future research could examine potential cause-and-effect relationships between leader use of power and forms of employee motivation using longitudinal data. Another potential limitation to this study is the use of single-source, self-report measures. Future research could include objective, observable outcomes to investigate how employee motivation and the use of various forms of leader power might impact organizational performance metrics.

Conclusion and Practical Implications

Overall, we found that leaders' use of hard power relates to higher levels of sub-optimal motivational outlooks in followers, while leaders' use of soft power is connected to higher levels of optimal motivational outlooks in followers. Followers' motivational outlooks were also related to their intentions to perform favorably for their organizations.

As this work provides insight into the relationship between leaders' use of power and less optimal kinds of follower motivation, we encourage managers in the field to consider how their use of coercive, legitimate, and reward power may be adversely connected to the daily quality of follower motivation. Said differently, leaders who often resort to hard types of power should proceed with caution, because they may unknowingly be undermining their own efforts to inspire an engaging and productive workplace that encourages autonomous regulation. Finally, we encourage both power and non-power holders to carefully consider how any use of power relates to the daily quality of follower motivation.

Ethics Statement

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. Namely, the Research Ethics Committee at the Ken Blanchard Companies approved the ethics of the study's design and procedures.

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study using the text provided to participants in the electronic survey's beginning instructions, as shown verbatim in italics below. The following informed consent text was adapted from a version of survey instruction text approved by the IRB through the University of San Diego for a 2012 study with similar instructions. For this study, the adapted instruction text was approved by the Research Ethics Committee at the Ken Blanchard Companies. We did not provide participants with a printed paper version of an informed consent form, due to the electronic format of the survey. However, participants indicated their voluntary agreement to participate by reading the below instructions and clicking forward into the survey. In that text, we made clear the researcher's promise to keep data anonymous and to only report individual data in aggregate form, and that participants could quit at any time without penalty.

“ The purpose of this questionnaire is to assess the extent to which certain leader behaviors impact employee work intentions. You are being asked to participate in a survey that will take about 15–20 min to complete. Completion of this survey involves no foreseeable risks. Your participation is voluntary and you may stop at any time with no penalty. No one will see your individual responses other than the researchers. Any data will be reported on a group basis only. You give your consent to participate in the study by completing this survey. If you have any questions please contact [email protected].”

For background, individuals invited to the survey had previously opted-in to receive electronic survey invitations of this kind, as their emails were being housed by a consulting company's national listserv database. This listserv and process has been used by The Ken Blanchard Companies to conduct survey research for the last 15 years, particularly in the areas of engagement and work passion for companies all over the globe. Protecting survey respondent confidentiality and ensuring participant psychological well-being has been a necessary precondition required by participating companies and individuals.

Author Contributions

TP led the data cleaning, data analysis, results write-up, and table/figure creation for both studies and is the corresponding author. DZ designed both studies, oversaw data collection, and wrote the literature review and discussion section. SF contributed to the literature review and discussion section, and all authors reviewed the piece prior to submission.

Conflict of Interest Statement

DZ is affiliated to the Ken Blanchard Companies through an advisory role, but he is not an employee of Blanchard. SF is Principal of Out of the Box Learning, Inc. (i.e., self-employed, author). TP is Principal of Valencore, LLC (i.e., at the time of this study she was self-employed). DZ and SF have both been employed by the University of San Diego for 20 years as adjuncts in the Master's of Executive Leadership program, where they teach two courses. TP is currently employed by Boston University's School of Hospitality Administration as a full-time faculty member.

  • Aguinis H., Nesler M. S., Quigley B. M., Tedeschi J. T. (1994). Perceptions of power: a cognitive perspective . Soc. Behav. Pers. 22 , 377–384. 10.2224/sbp.1994.22.4.377 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ajzen I., Fishbein M. (1980). Understanding Attitudes and Predicting Social Behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Anderson C., Brion S. (2014). Perspectives on power in organizations . Annu. Rev. Organ. Psychol. Organ. Behav. 1 , 67–97. 10.1146/annurev-orgpsych-031413-09125 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Armitage C. J., Connor M. (2001). Efficacy of the theory of planned behavior: a meta-analytic review . Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 40 , 471–500. 10.1348/014466601164939 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bagozzi R. P. (1992). The self-regulation of attitudes, intentions, and behavior . Soc. Psychol. Q. 55 , 178–204. 10.2307/2786945 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Chan D. (2009). So why ask me? Are self-report data really that bad? in Statistical and Methodological Myths and Urban Legends: Doctrine, Verity and Fable in the Organizational and Social Sciences , eds Lance C. E., Vandenberg R. J. (New York, NY: Routledge; ), 309–336. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Clegg R., Courpasson D., Phillips N. (2006). Power and Organizations . Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Conger J. A., Kanungo R. N. (1988). The empowerment process: Integrating theory and practice . Acad. Manage. Rev. 13 , 471–482. 10.5465/AMR.1988.430-6983 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cooke R., Sheeran P. (2004). Moderation of cognition-intention and cognition-behavior relations: a meta-analysis of properties of the variables from the theory of planned behavior . Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 43 , 159–186. 10.1348/0144666041501688 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dambe M., Moorad F. (2008). From power to empowerment: a paradigm shift in leadership . S. Afr. J. Higher Educ. 22 , 575–587. 10.4314/sajhe.v22i3.25803 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • DeCelles K. A., DeRue D. S., Margolis J. D., Ceranic T. L. (2012). Does power corrupt or enable? When and why power facilitates self-interested behavior . J. Appl. Psychol. 97 , 681–689. 10.1037/a0026811 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Deci E. L. (1980). The Psychology of Self-Determination. Lexington, MA: Lexington books. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Deci E. L., Ryan R. M. (1985). Intrinsic Motivation and Self-Determination in Human Behavior . New York, NY: Plenum. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Deci E. L., Ryan R. M. (2002). Self-determination research: reflections future directions , in Handbook of Self-Determination Research , eds Deci E. L., Ryan R. M. (Rochester, NY: University Rochester Press; ), 431–441. [ Google Scholar ]
  • DuBrin A. J. (2009). Political Behavior in Organizations. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Duffy R. D., Lent R. W. (2009). Test of the social cognitive model of work satisfaction . J. Vocat. Behav. 75 , 212–223. 10.1016/j.jvb.2009.06.001 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dysvik A., Kuvaas B. (2010). Exploring the relative and combined influence of mastery-approach goals and work intrinsic motivation on employee turnover intention . Pers. Rev. 39 , 622–638. 10.1108/00483481011064172 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Elangovan A. R., Xie J. L. (1999). Effects of perceived power of supervisor on subordinates stress and motivation: the moderating role of subordinate characteristics . J. Organ. Behav. 20 , 359–373. 10.1002/(SIC)1099-1379(199905)20:3<359::AID-JOB902>3.0.CO;2-Z [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Elias S. (2008). Fifty years of influence in the workplace: the evolution of the French and Raven power taxonomy . J. Manage. Hist. 14 , 267–283. 10.1108/17511340810880634 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Farmer S. M., Aguinis H. (2005). Accounting for subordinate perceptions of supervisory power: an identity-dependence model . J. Appl. Psychol. 90 , 1069–1083. 10.1037/0021-9010.906.1069 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Folkman S., Lazarus R. S., Dunkel-Schetter C., DeLongis A., Gruen R. T. (1986). Dynamics of a stressful encounter: cognitive appraisal, coping, and encounter outcomes . J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 50 , 992–1003. 10.1037/0022-3514.50.5.992 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Foucault M. (1982). The subject and power . Crit. Inq. 8 , 777–795. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fowler S. (2014). Why Motivating People Doesn't Work … and What Does: The New Science of Leading, Energizing and Engaging . San Francisco, CA: Berrett-Koehler. [ Google Scholar ]
  • French J., Raven's B. H. (1959). The bases of social power , in Studies in Social Power , ed Cartwright D. (Ann Arbor, MI: Institute for Social Research; ), 150–167. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fugate M., Harrison S., Kinicki A. J. (2011). Thoughts and feelings about organizational change: a field test of appraisal theory . J. Leadersh. Organ. Stud. 18 , 421–437. 10.1177/1548051811416510 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gagné M. (2016), June. What's wrong with introjection? in Paper Presented at the 6th International Conference on Self-Determination Theory (Victoria, BC: ). [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gagné M., Deci E. L. (2005). Self-determination and work motivation . J. Organ. Behav. 26 , 331–362. 10.1002/job.314 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gagné M., Forest J., Vansteenkiste M., Crevier-Braud L., Van den Broeck A., Aspeli A. K., et al. (2015). The multidimensional work motivation scale: validation evidence in seven languages and nine countries . Eur. J. Work Organ. Psychol. 24 , 178–196. 10.1080/135-9432X.2013.877892 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Galinsky A. D., Magee J. C., Inesi M. E., Gruenfeld D. H. (2006). Power and perspectives not taken . Psychol. Sci. 17 , 1068–1074. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01824.x [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gollwitzer P. M., Paschal S. (2006). Implementation intentions and goal achievement: a meta-analysis of affection processes . Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 38 , 69–119. 10.1016/S0065-2601(06)38002-1 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gotlieb J. D., Grewal D., Brown S. W. (1994). Consumer satisfaction and preferred quality: complementary or diverging constructs? J. Appl. Psychol. 79 , 875–885. 10.1037/0021-9010.79.6.875 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Haugaard M., Clegg S. (2012). Power and Organizations . Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hooper D., Coughlan J., Mullen M. R. (2008). Structural equation modelling: guidelines for determining model fit . Electron. J. Bus. Res. Methods 6 , 53–60. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hu L., Bentler P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives . Struct. Equat. Model. 6 , 1–55. 10.1080/10705519909540118 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jungert T., Koestner R.F., Houlfort N., Schattke K. (2013). Distinguishing sources of autonomy support in relation to worker's motivation and self-efficacy . J. Soc. Psychol. 153 , 651–666. 10.1080/00224545.2013.806292 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lammers J., Stapel D. A. (2009). How power influences moral thinking . J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 97 , 279–289. 10.1037/a0015437 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lukes S. (2005). Power and the battle for hearts and minds . Millennium J. Int. Stud. 33 , 477–494. 10.1177/03058298050330031201 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lukes S. (1974). Power: A Radical View . London: Macmillan. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lunenberg F. C. (2012). Power and leadership: an influence process . Int. J. Manage. Bus. Admin. 15 , 1–9. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Meyer J. P., Gagné M., Parfyonova M. (2010). Toward an evidence-based model of engagement: What we can learn for motivation and commitment research , in Handbook of Employee Engagement: Perspectives, Issues, Research and Practice , ed Albrecht S. (North Hampton, MA: Edward Elgar: ), 62–73. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mossholder K. W., Bennett N., Kemery E. R., Wesolowski M. A. (1998). Relationships between the bases of power and work reactions: the mediational role of procedural justice . J. Manage. 24 , 533–552. 10.1177/014920639802400404 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nimon K., Zigarmi D. (2015). The work intention inventory–short form . N. Horiz. Adult Educ. Hum. Res. Dev. 27 , 15–27. 10.1002/nha3.20090 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Patall E. A., Cooper H., Robinson J. C. (2008). The effects of choice on intrinsic motivation and related outcomes: a meta-analysis of research findings . Psychol. Bull. 134 , 270–300. 10.1037/0033-2909.134.2.270 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pfeffer J. (1992). Managing With Power: Politics and Influence in Organizations . Boston, MA: Harvard Business School Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pfeffer J. (2011). Power: Why Some People Have it and Others Don't. New York, NY: HarperCollins. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pierro A., Cicero L., Raven B. H. (2008). Motivated compliance with bases of social power . J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 38 , 1921–1944. 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2008.00374.x [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pierro A., Kruglanski A. W., Raven H. B. (2012). Motivational underpinnings of social influence in work settings: bases of social power and the need for cognitive closure . Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 42 , 41–52. 10.1002/ejsp.836 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pierro A., Raven B. H., Amato C., Belanger J. J. (2013). Bases of social power, leadership styles, and organizational commitment . Int. J. Psychol. 48 , 1122–1134. 10.1080/00207594.2012.733398 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Podsakoff N. P., LePine J. A., LePine M. A. (2007). Differential challenge stressor-hindrance stressor relationships with job attitudes, turnover intentions, turnover, and withdrawal behavior: a meta-analysis . J. Appl. Psychol. 92 , 438–454. 10.1037/0021-9010.92.2.438 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Podsakoff P.M., MacKenzie S.B., Lee Y., Podsakoff N.P. (2003). Common method biases in behavioral research: a critical review of the literature and recommended remedies . J. Appl. Psychol. 88 , 879–903. 10.1037/0021-9010.88.5.879 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Podsakoff P. M., Organ D. W. (1986). Self-reports in organizational research: problems and prospects . J. Manage. 12 , 531–544. 10.1177/014920638601200408 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Podsakoff P. M., Schriesheim C. A. (1985). Field studies of French and Raven's bases of power: critique, reanalysis, and suggestions for future research . Psychol. Bull. 97 , 387–411. 10.1037/0033-2909.97.3.387 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Politis J. D. (2005). The influence of managerial power and credibility on knowledge acquisition attributes . Leadersh. Organ. Dev. J. 26 , 197–114. 10.1108/01437730510591752 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Randolph W. A., Kemery E. R. (2011). Managerial use of power bases in a model of managerial empowerment practices and employee psychological empowerment . J. Leadersh. Organ. Stud. 18 , 95–106. 10.1177/1548051810379798 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Raven B. H. (1965). Social influence and power , in Current Studies in Social Psychology , eds Steiner I. D., Fishbein M. (New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart & Winston; ), 371–381. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Raven B. H. (1993). The basis of power: origins and recent developments . J. Soc. Behav. Pers. 7 , 217–244. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Raven B. H., Schwarzwald J., Kozlowsky M. (1998). Conceptualizing and measuring a power/interaction model of interpersonal influence . J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 28 , 307–332. 10.1111/j1559-1816.1998.tb01707.x [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ryan R. M., Deci E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development and wellbeing . Am. Psychol. 55 , 68–73. 10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.68 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ryan R. M., Deci E. L. (2017). Self-Determination Theory: Basic Psychological Needs in Motivation Development and Wellness . New York, NY: John Wiley. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Spector P. (2006). Method variance in organizational research: truth or urban legend? Organ. Res. Methods 9 , 221–232. 10.1177/1094428105284955 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Steel R. P., Ovalle K. (1984). A review and meta-analysis of research on the relationship between behavioral intentions and employee turnover . J. Appl. Psychol. 69 , 273–286. 10.1037/002-9010.69.4.673 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Stone D. N., Deci E. L., Ryan R. M. (2009). Beyond talk: creating autonomous motivation through self-determination theory . J. Gen. Manage. 34 , 75–91. 10.1177/030630700903400305 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Strum R. E., Antonakis J. (2015). Interpersonal power: a review, critique, and research agenda . J. Manage. 41 , 136–163. 10.1177/0149206314555769 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Tett R. P., Meyer J. K. (1993). Job satisfaction, organizational commitment, turnover intention, and turnover: path analysis based on meta-analytic findings . Pers. Psychol. 46 , 259–293. 10.1111/j.1744-6570.1993.tb00874.x [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Vince R. (2014). What do HRD scholars and practitioners need to know about power, emotion, and HRD? Hum. Res. Dev. Q. 25 , 409–420. 10.1002/hrdq.21191 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ward E. A. (1998). Managerial power bases subordinate manifest needs as influences on psychological climate . J. Bus. Psychol. 12 , 361–378. 10.1023/A:1025083631828 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Webb T. L., Sheeran P. (2006). Does changing behavioral intentions engender behavioral change? A meta-analysis of the experimental evidence . Psychol. Bull. 132 , 249–268. 10.1037/0033-2909.132.2.249 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Williams L. J., Hartman N., Cavazotte F. (2010). Method variance and marker variables: a review and comprehensive CFA marker technique . Organ. Res. Methods 13 , 477–514. 10.1177/1094428110366036 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Yukl G., Tracy J. B. (1992). Consequences of influence bases used with subordinates, peers, and boss . J. Appl. Psychol. 77 , 525–535. 10.1037/0021-9010.77.4.525 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Zigarmi D., Galloway F., Roberts T. P. (2016). Work locus of control, motivational regulation, employee work passion and work intentions: an empirical investigation of an appraisal model . J. Happ. S. 19 , 231–256. 10.1007/s10902-016-9813-2 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Zigarmi D., Nimon K., Houson D., Witt D., Diehl J. (2012). The work intention inventory: initial evidence of construct validity . J. Bus. Admin. Res. 1 , 24–42. 10.5430/jbar.vinlp24 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Zigarmi D., Roberts T. P., Randolph W. (2015). Employees'perceived use of leader power and implications for affect and work intentions . Hum. Res. Dev. Q. 26 , 359–384. 10.1002/hrdq.21216 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]

IMAGES

  1. (PDF) Chaos and the Abuse of Power: Workplace Bullying in

    research paper on abuse of power

  2. 004 Police Abuse Of Power Essay Example Domestic Violence Topics

    research paper on abuse of power

  3. (DOC) Research Proposal Drug abuse 3

    research paper on abuse of power

  4. Abuse of Power Research Paper Example

    research paper on abuse of power

  5. The Abuse of Power

    research paper on abuse of power

  6. The Abuse of Power : Anthony Daly : 9781912624294 : Blackwell's

    research paper on abuse of power

COMMENTS

  1. Research Paper Abuse of power: An experimental investigation of the effects of power and transparency on centralized punishment☆

    So far we have only seen how many subjects decided to abuse, but we have not seen the amount of abuse. As can be seen in Fig. 1 there seems to be no level effect of transparency in the low-power treatment on amount abused (t(46) = 0.9, p = 0.39, d= 0.2 (small)) while there is a level effect of transparency in the high-power treatment (t(46) = 2.7, p = 0.011, d= 0.8 (medium)).

  2. On power and its corrupting effects: the effects of power on human

    Even with increased knowledge of power's corrupting effect and safeguards put in place to counteract such tendencies, power abuse remains rampant in society suggesting that the full extent of this effect is not well understood. In this paper, an effort is made to improve understanding of power's corrupting effects on human behavior through ...

  3. Bullying and the Abuse of Power

    By recognizing and defining school bullying as an abuse of power and a violation of human rights, Olweus has laid the foundation and created the impetus for researching and addressing bullying. This review highlights the importance of examining abuses of power not only in school relationships, but across human relationships and society in general.

  4. Demystifying the academy: Resistance, ethics and abuse of power

    Given that the purpose of this study is to examine the abuse of power in academia, the theoretical framework draws upon auto-ethnographic research (Anderson, 2006; Bochner and Ellis, 2016; Adams et al., 2014).Auto-ethnography 'refers to a particular form of writing that seeks to unite ethnography (looking outward at a world beyond one's own) and autobiographical (gazing inward for a story ...

  5. Identifying and Mitigating the Negative Effects of Power in

    Abstract. This article investigates the possible negative effects of power in organizations. It demonstrates how holding power may affect an individual's perception of others, actions, and cognition. Ways in which power may have a detrimental effect on different aspects of a manager's functioning within an organization, including building ...

  6. Bullying and the Abuse of Power

    Dan Olweus pioneered research on school bullying and identified the importance of, and risk factors associated with, bullying and victimization. In this paper, we conduct a narrative review of the critical notion of power within bullying. Specifically, we discuss Olweus's definition of bullying and the role of a power imbalance in distinguishing bullying behavior from other forms of ...

  7. On power and its corrupting effects: the effects of power on human

    2.1. Power is addictive. There is evidence of addiction to the power derived from celebrity and fame [Citation 56].The addictive effect on the powerholder promotes the need to engage in efforts to hold on to and accumulate power [Citation 57-59].Aging, envy, and fear both conscious and unconscious of retaliation for previous acts may contribute to power's addictiveness [Citation 58].

  8. (PDF) Bullying and the Abuse of Power

    By recognizing and defining school bullying as an abuse of power and a violation of human rights, Olweus has laid the foundation and created the impetus for researching and addressing bullying.

  9. PDF Bullying and the Abuse of Power

    the abuse of power that extends beyond the school context and emerges within families, workplaces, and governments. By recognizing and dening school bullying as an abuse of power and a violation of human rights, Olweus has laid the founda-tion and created the impetus for researching and addressing bullying. This review highlights the importance ...

  10. Abuse Practice of Power in Orwell's Animal Farm

    practice of power in Animal Farm. I n this research paper, "The Abuse of Power in George Orwell's Animal Farm" we. pinpointed the twentieth century modern pe riod and modern novels as the ...

  11. (PDF) Abuse of Power

    Abuse of Power. October 2008; Journal of Evidence-Based Social ... Developed a peer nomination scale to assess the degree to which children are subjected to direct physical and verbal abuse by ...

  12. Abuse of Power

    We investigate power abuse of a single punisher in a public-goods-game subject to variations in punishment power and contribution transparency. We find a high amount of abuse across all conditions. More power led to more abuse over time, while transparency could only curb abuse in the high power conditions.

  13. Privatising Human Rights and the Abuse of Power

    Privatising Human Rights and the Abuse of Power - Volume 13 Issue 1. ... Fundamental Legal Conceptions as Applied in Judicial Reasoning and Other Legal Essays (New Haven, CT: Yale University Press, 1923). In the context of the relations between one individual and another a liberty, unlike a right, does not impose a correlative duty on the other

  14. PDF Measures to Protect Victims of Crime and The Abuse of Power in ...

    To recognize that crime and abuse of power victimizations are significant human experiences, mostly the result of ineffective public policies, the adversarial justice model and cultural support for the use of violence in personal and collective problem solving. 2. To emphasize that crime and abuse of power victimizations have caused extensive ...

  15. Power Day: Addressing the Use and Abuse of Power in Medical Training

    Approach. Beginning in 2001, Yale School of Medicine has held annual "Power Day" workshops for third year medical students and advanced practice nursing students, to define and analyse power dynamics within the medical hierarchy and hidden curriculum using literature, guest speakers, and small groups. During rotations, medical students ...

  16. Manifestations of power abuse in academia and how to prevent them

    In recent years, evidence of cases of power abuse in academia have been made public in various news reports about poor leadership, bullying and even sexual harassment of employees by superiors at universities (Boytchev, 2020; Hartocollis, 2018; Schenkel, 2018) and non-university research organisations in Germany and abroad (Devlin & Marsh, 2018 ...

  17. On power and its corrupting effects: the effects of power on human

    Even with increased knowledge of power's corrupting effect and safeguards put in place to counteract such tendencies, power abuse remains rampant in society suggesting that the full extent of this effect is not well understood. In this paper, an effort is made to improve understanding of power's corrupting effects on human behavior through an ...

  18. On power and its corrupting effects: the effects of power on human

    On power and its corrupting effects: the effects of power on human behavior and the limits of accountability systems Tobore Onojighofia Tobore ... power abuse remains rampant in society suggesting that the full extent of this effect is not well understood. In this paper, an effort is made to improve understanding of power's corrupting effects ...

  19. Abuse of Power depicted in Animal Farm Research Paper

    View PDF. Senanur Kır Deniz Ezgi Avcı Vile UNI 123 05 Analyse the reasons for abuse of power depicted in Animal Farm. As John Acton observed, "Power tends to corrupt, and absolute power corrupts absolutely.". In his novel Animal Farm, George Orwell portrays a corrupted animal community that he parallelized with our society.

  20. Examining the Relationship Between Leaders' Power Use, Followers

    The term power is most commonly defined as "the asymmetric control over valued resources" (Anderson and Brion, 2014, p. 69). Power relationships are inherently social and exist only in relation to others; parties with low power rely on parties with high power to obtain rewards and avoid punishment (Vince, 2014).

  21. (PDF) Corruption and Abuse of Power: A Reflection of ...

    This is vividly seen in the works of three Minangkabau writers. Their exposure. of social issues, such as corruption and abuse of pow er, shows great c oncern on the. matter. As a reflection of ...

  22. Abuse Of Power Essays: Examples, Topics, & Outlines

    Harassment: Verbal, physical, or emotional abuse from those in positions of power can create a hostile work environment and harm employees' well-being. 4. Retaliation: When employees fear retaliation for speaking up about unethical behavior or reporting misconduct, it can lead to a culture of silence and cover-ups. 5.

  23. (PDF) Abuse of power by law enforcement authorities in India with

    Abuse of power by law enforcement authorities in India 13 26 Surendranath, A. (2020) 'Police violence and how some lives d o not matter', The Hindu, (updated on 4 July 2020).